The Subtle Psychology of Voter Turnout

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "The Subtle Psychology of Voter Turnout"

Transcription

1 The Subtle Psychology of Voter Turnout Daniel G. Goldstein, 1 Kosuke Imai, 2 Anja S. Göritz 3 1 London Business School, UK 2 Princeton University, USA 3 University of Erlangen-Nuremberg, Germany To whom correspondence should be addressed: dgoldstein@london.edu March 26, 2007 Randomized experiments conducted during the 2006 US midterm election and the 2005 German federal election were analyzed in order to estimate the turnout effects of two simple treatments: asking people if and how they intend to vote. Since World War II, in over 1,600 national elections in 170 independent states, voter turnout rates have averaged about 65% of the voting age population (1, 2). Policy makers in 18% of these democracies have deemed electoral participation important enough to justify compulsory voting laws, under which non-voters can face fines and other punishments. Recently, the US Congress authorized 3.9 billion dollars for the Help America Vote Act, and state governments have invested in expanding early-voting methods, which accounted for roughly 20% of the votes cast in the 2004 election (3, 4). Worldwide, rewards for voters have included tax breaks, job opportunities, scholarships, and even high-stakes lotteries (1). Alongside government campaigns are partisan ones: US Democrats and Republicans spent roughly 100 million dollars on selective turnout programs in the 2000 election alone (5). 1 Electronic copy of this paper is available at:

2 What drives voter turnout? Political theory speaks of the costs and benefits of voting and the slight probability that one s vote will be decisive (6). In practice, these and other variables appear in policies that target two causes of weak participation: low motivation and high obstacles (7). Motivation-focused initiatives aim to impart the desire to vote by invoking the importance or closeness of an election, a voter s sense of duty, rewards, punishments, or social comparisons. Obstacle-focused policies aim to make voting easier, such as by introducing same-day or automatic registration, voting by mail, or early in-person voting. If voting is largely influenced by motivations and obstacles, policy makers might take inspiration from psychological research on goal attainment, which has revealed the strong effects of two simple treatments. In this paper, we demonstrate that simply asking people if and how they intend to vote can increase turnout. The technique of asking people if they intend to vote comes from research on attitude accessibility and self-fulfilling prediction. In what is called the mere measurement or questionbehavior effect (8,9), people become more likely to perform certain actions if they are first asked whether they expect to perform them. That is, merely measuring intentions changes behavior. One surprising study found that asking people whether they intended to buy an automobile increased their chances of doing so (9). Why does mere measurement work? One important literature suggests that people who make forecasts about the future may alter their behavior to make the predictions come true (10). An emerging and complementary view is that when people answer questions about intentions, their underlying attitudes become concrete and readily accessible (9,11). For this reason, questions can be polarizing. If underlying attitudes are positive, mere measurement can increase the likelihood of performing an action if negative, the same questions can decrease it. If attitudes toward electoral participation are generally positive, assessing intentions may turn voting into a goal. 2 Electronic copy of this paper is available at:

3 Eighty years of research has looked at the effect of polls, questions, and surveys on voter turnout ( (12) for a review, see (13)). For instance, twenty years ago, Greenwald and colleagues (14) found some promising but tentative results on a sample of sixty college students. However, theirs, like many if not all others in the literature, was not a pure test of mere measurement, as participants were not just asked about intentions, but were also provided information about where and how to register to vote if they did not know. Over the last 80 years, research has left an unclear picture of the effects of mere-measurement on voting due to mixed results and some methodological controversies (13, 15). Part of the variation in results may be due to the variety of populations, instruments, and historical periods studied. Additional variation may be due to the way experiments have mixed mere-measurement treatments with related political questions and even practical information for voters. The second technique, asking people how they intend to vote, comes from research on implementation intentions, which are simple plans that help people overcome obstacles en route to goal attainment. The effects of implementation intentions have been estimated in over 100 policy-relevant studies on exercising, recycling, smoking, and beyond, however, the link to voting has not been investigated in the literature (16). How do implementation intentions work? These plans are hypothesized to lead one to direct resources (such as time and attention) toward a target goal, and away from competing goals when they inevitably arise. Furthermore, implementation intentions might make one aware of goal-realization opportunities that would otherwise go unnoticed (e.g., noticing registration offices near work), and help automate responses to foreseeable obstacles (e.g., identifying a means of backup transportation to the polls) (16). We illustrate the application of mere measurement and implementation intentions through randomized experiments, analyzed in order to estimate causal effects on voter turnout in two national elections: the 2006 US Midterm Election and the 2005 German Federal Election. 3

4 We present here a summary methodological details are found in the appendix. In the US study, 1, 968 voting-age members of a nationwide research panel were invited to take part in a brief survey approximately two months before the election. In it, a mere measurement group was asked about intentions to vote, and an implementation intentions group was additionally asked to formulate simple plans to vote. A control group completed a filler task to equalize time. The crucial difference with the German study, which invited 1, 426 people, is that it took place 1 to 4 days before the election, presumably leaving treatments fresh in the minds of participants. After the election, a follow-up study measured turnout (election day voting and early voting). As detailed in the appendix, we use a diary technique to validate US election day voting and make statistical adjustments for non-random dropout in both experiments. The experiments pose novel theoretical and applied questions. Will implementation intentions have an effect above that of mere measurement? Will the two treatments be effective on one-shot goals (17) that can be realized only on one day (e.g., voting on Election Day) and open-ended goals that can be realized on many possible days (e.g., early and postal voting)? For both types of goals, do mere measurement and implementation intentions treatments fade over periods of days or months? The methodological challenge of these experiments is that not all people participate in follow-up studies. Although the problem of such drop-out is common in survey experiments (18), the main concern here is that the drop-out probability may directly depend on the voting behavior: those who vote may be more likely to report their voting behavior (19). The methodology we develop in the appendix material allows for the drop-out probability to directly depend on the (possibly unobserved) outcome variable, as well as various pre-treatment covariates measured for each subject, thereby yielding valid statistical inference (20). [Figure 1 about here.] 4

5 The results of the experiments are displayed in Figure 1. For the open-ended goal of early (e.g., postal) voting, mere measurement treatments given two months in advance (US study) had moderate positive effects on turnout, a finding consistent with studies showing that mere measurement treatments can impact the probability of undertaking an action (such as purchasing a computer) on any day within a window of several months (9). For early voting, estimated implementation intentions effects were similar to those of mere measurement, though 2.7 percentage points greater. For the one-shot goal of election-day voting, mere measurement was only effective when it was administered days (Germany), but not months (US) in advance. Implementation intentions treatments, in contrast, held their effectiveness for both near and distant races. Policy makers seeking to craft inexpensive and unobtrusive ways of increasing turnout will note that mere measurement was about as effective as implementation intentions for all conditions except distant, one-shot goals. However, 14% of those who were assigned to the US implementation intentions group refused to write plans while only 3% of the mere measurement group declined to provide intentions. If the compliance rate was higher in the implementation intentions group, the actual treatment effect might have been larger. Nevertheless, our estimates are relevant to policy since a similar pattern of noncompliance is expected to occur in practice. Our study contributes to a growing body of research demonstrating that policies can benefit from working in concert with psychological mechanisms. People s preference for default options, for instance, can lead to increased membership in organ donor pools (21), and participation in retirement savings plans (22). While some policies benefit from a tendency toward inaction, others must help people to act. To construct effective campaigns and messages, policy makers might consider addressing voting as a goal, one that is aided by stating intentions and making plans. 5

6 Appendix 2006 US Midterm Election Experiment Participants were 2,469 members of the London Business School Online Laboratory Panel, selected from the database for being US citizens of voting age. The experiment consisted of two phases. The first phase ran from September 15th to 25th, 2006, some seven to eight weeks before the election of November 7th, The second phase took place from November 8th to 11th, 2006, one to four days after the election. Before the first phase, participants were randomly assigned to three groups of equal size; a control group, mere measurement (MM) group, and implementation intentions (II) group. Members of all groups were sent identical s inviting them to participate in an online study on decision making. Every participant was offered payment of one US dollar in addition to entry in a lottery in which one participant would receive 100 dollars, five participants would receive 20 dollars, and 10 participants would receive 10 dollars. To improve the efficiency of the resulting causal estimates, we used a matched-pair design where complete randomization of the treatments was conducted within each group of three observations with similar characteristics (18, 23). These randomized groups are formed based on the Mahalanobis distance of the selected pre-treatment variables, which is a common metric used to measure the degree of similarity among observations in terms of those multidimensional characteristics. The variables we used are years in residence, gender, age, age squared, marital status, employment status (full-time or not), whether a respondent has at least one child, whether a respondent is most fluent in English, annual income, and years of education. From this initial sample, we first exclude 62 invitees who had non-functioning addresses or inconsistent demographic information. The group sizes for the control, MM, and 6

7 II groups are reduced to 800, 809, 798, respectively. (As a convention, groups will always be listed in this order.) Next, we exclude those who did not begin the experiment at all, measured by agreeing to a consent page that is identical across conditions. This yields group sizes of 400, 430, and 379 for the final sample we analyze. The above exclusion criteria are based on the pretreatment information that could not be affected by the treatment conditions, thus any variation across the groups is assumed to have arisen at random. As expected, the observed covariates are well balanced between the three groups. For example, Pearson s χ 2 tests show that the observed differences in gender, marital status, and employment status are not statistically significant. During the first phase, the control group provided basic demographic information, then completed a five-minute filler task, used to equalize task time with the other conditions. The MM group was identical to the control, except that before the filler task, it was asked to indicate the strength of voting intentions by rating the following statements from Dholakia and Bagozzi (17) on a 9 point scale ranging from I agree completely to I disagree completely : I intend to vote in the upcoming US Midterm Election, I am very committed to voting in the upcoming US Midterm Election, and It would not take much for me to abandon my goal of voting in the upcoming US Midterm Election. The II group was the same as the MM group but without the filler task and with the addition of items used to elicit implementation intentions, also adapted from Dholakia and Bagozzi. The items were the following three questions, each followed by a text entry box on the Web form: 1) If you are not registered to vote, please write a few sentences answering the following questions by listing specific steps. How will you find out about registering? When will you find out about registering? When will you register? Where will you register? How will you register?, 2) Listing specific steps, please write a few sentences answering the following questions. How will you vote (in person or by mail)? Where will you look for information on voting? If you vote in person, how will you find out where to vote? When will you find out your voting location? 7

8 If you will vote by mail, how will you find out about postal voting? When will you find out about postal voting?, and 3) Listing specific steps, please write a few sentences answering the following questions. If you vote in person, what time of day will you go to vote? Where will you vote? How will you get to your voting location? If you vote by mail, when will you mail your ballot? Where will you mail it from? Each of the three items was followed by a question asking for a contingency plan of the following kind: Sometimes a good plan prevents a difficult situation from happening. If you are not registered to vote, please write a few sentences about what might go wrong with this plan for registering to vote. What will you do if these obstacles arise? What can you do to prevent them from arising? People not intending to vote were instructed to answer as if they did intend. At no point in the experiment was the possibility of a follow-up study mentioned. To avoid the problem of deception in self-reports (24), the following diary method was used to measure in-person voting in the four days after the election, some two months after the treatment. Diary methods offer a complementary line of evidence to the method of checking votes against electoral rolls, which has some drawbacks, such as limiting the sample to people already in voter registration databases (not allowing for the possibility of registering as a result of the experiment), limiting the sample to select counties (as opposed to the entire country), and missing lost votes (due to faulty equipment, registration mix ups, polling place operations, lost absentee ballots) of which there were 4 to 6 million in the 2000 US Presidential election (25). All the original invitees received a seemingly unrelated boilerplate invitation about a short research study on memory. Upon starting the memory study, participants were informed that they would be asked to remember, in as much detail as possible, what they did hour by hour on a particular day. To illustrate the task, they were shown an example diary with items such as 7AM - Drove to the Lazy Daisy Cafe and 12PM - Had a cheese sandwich for lunch in the cafeteria. Participants were then given a blank diary with fields for each hour from 5AM 8

9 through 11PM, and were told which day to remember. Each person actually received the same day, Tuesday, and no mention was made of it being the midterm election day. After completing the diaries, they were submitted irrevocably online. At this point, participants were asked if they voted in person on Tuesday, voted before Tuesday (by post or early in-person voting), or did not vote. All participants were paid one dollar and fifty cents for participation, plus entry in a small stakes lottery. In the analysis phase, respondents were coded as having voted in person if and only if they listed having voted in their diaries. The number of participants who agreed to participate in the second phase in the control, MM, and II groups was 242, 260, and 207, respectively. In this experiment, the possibility of non-random dropout is an important methodological issue and is addressed in our statistical analysis German Federal Election Experiment Participants were members of two German Web panels, Promio.net and WiSo-Panel (26). The experiment consisted of two phases. The first phase ran from September 13th to 17th, 2005, one to five days before the election of September 18th, The second phase took place from September 19th to 22nd, 2005, one to four days after the election. Before the first phase, panel database members were randomly assigned to the control group, mere measurement group, or implementation intentions group. Members of all groups were sent identical s inviting them to participate in an online study on decision making. For an analysis not relevant to the focus of this paper, participants were either paid by the panels, paid 1.5 Euros by our lab, or decided to take part in the experiment without pay. Web links in 252, 584, and 590 invitations were clicked on. (The latter two treatment conditions were allowed to gather more responses in the interest of detecting differences between them.) Of these, 249, 579, and 586 agreed to a consent page. This is the sample we analyze in this paper. During the first phase, the control group provided basic demographic information. The MM 9

10 group was identical to the control group except that it was given a yes-no item about intention to vote. The II group was identical to the MM group, except that it was given two additional items. The first asked those intending to vote to list one main obstacle that might prevent them from voting. The second requested that they write a plan they could use to overcome this obstacle should it arise. In the second phase, after the election, participants were asked whether they voted in person, voted by mail, or did not vote. All respondents were invited to participate after the election, and 204, 485, and 512 individuals consented. Similar to the US election experiment, the possibility of non-random dropout is addressed in our statistical analysis. Statistical Methodology Finally, we describe the statistical methodology used to analyze our two experiments. Let T i be the multi-valued treatment variable where T i J = {0, 1,...,J 1}. In our experiments, J is equal to 3 the control, MM, and II groups but we maintain the general notation throughout this section. We develop our methodology under the potential outcomes framework of causal inference commonly used in statistics (27). Let Y i (t) {0, 1} denote the potential turnout variable of voter i under the scenario that the voter receives the treatment value t J. For example, Y i (0) = 1 means that if voter i is assigned to the control group, he/she would vote in the election. For each voter, we only observe one of the J potential outcome variables, and this observed outcome variable is denoted by Y i = J 1 j=0 1(T i = j)y i (j) where 1( ) is the indicator function. The fundamental problem of causal inference is that any causal effect involves at least two potential outcomes, e.g., Y i (1) Y i (0), and hence can never be directly observed. To address the issue of non-random dropout in our experiments, we use R i (T i ) {0, 1} to represent the potential binary recording variable which equals 1 if voter i would report actual voting behavior in the post-election survey after receiving the treatment value of T i (and 0 otherwise). There are J potential recording variables for each voter, but as with the potential 10

11 turnout variable, we only observe one of them. We denote the observed recording variable R i = J 1 j=0 1(T i = j)r i (j). We also observe some covariates for each participant such as age and gender, which are measured in the pre-election survey and denoted by X i. The quantity of interest we report in Figure 1 is the sample average treatment effect or ATE for each treatment, τ j = 1 n Y i (j) Y i (0), (1) n i=1 for j = 1,...,J 1. Since the treatment assignment is randomized, the treatment variable T i is independent of all potential outcomes, the estimation of the ATE is straightforward if the outcome variable is observed for every participant. However, in our experiments, some voters did not participate in the post-election survey and hence the outcome variable Y i was not recorded for them. This means that the standard complete-case analysis yields a biased estimate of the ATE unless the drop-out behavior is generated completely at random, which is unlikely in this case. To make more credible inferences, one possibility is to assume that the data are missing at random (MAR). That is, a voter s decision to report voting behavior the post-election survey may depend on the treatment he/she received, but given the received treatment and observed covariates, the decision is assumed to be independent of whether he/she voted in the election, i.e., Pr(R i (j) = 1 T i = j, Y i (j) = 0, X i ) = Pr(R i (j) = 1 T i = j, Y i (j) = 1, X i ) for all j J. Although the MAR assumption may be reasonable in some situations, it is problematic in our experiments because those who voted in the election are more likely to report their voting behavior (19). Thus, we allow for the possibility that a voter s decision to answer the post-election survey in our experiments may depend on his/her voting behavior itself by assuming that treatment assignments affect the response decision only indirectly through the voting behavior. The filler tasks given to the control and MM groups in the pre-election 11

12 survey are intended to equalize the survey time, thereby minimizing the possibility of the direct effects of the treatments on the dropout mechanism. Formally, the assumption is called the non-ignorability (NI) assumption and can be written as, Pr(R i (j) = 1 T i = j, Y i (j) = k,x i ) = Pr(R i (j ) = 1 T i = j, Y i (j ) = k,x i ), (2) for all j j and k = 0, 1. It has been shown that under the NI assumption, the average treatment effects are identified (20) (see also (28)). To estimate the ATE, we conduct a Bayesian inference under the NI assumption in Equation 2 by following the estimation strategies described in (20). In particular, we model the turnout and missing data mechanism using the two probit regressions, p j (z) Pr(Y i = 1 T i = j, Z i = z) = Φ(α j + β z), (3) r k (x) Pr(R i = 1 Y i = k,x i = x) = Φ(δ k + γ x), (4) for j J and k = 0, 1 where Z i is a subset of X i as explained below and Φ( ) represents the cumulative distribution function of the standard normal random variable. Equation 2 suggests that in order for the NI assumption to hold, it is important to control for relevant confounding covariates in the response model of Equation 4 (18). The key variable we use for this purpose is the vote intention from the pre-election survey, which was measured for those in the MM and II groups. We also include an indicator variable for the voters whose vote intention variable was not observed (i.e., those in the MM and II groups who did not answer this question as well as everyone in the control group). These variables are not included in the outcome model of Equation 3, however, because they constitute a part of the treatments of interest. Furthermore, we include several pre-treatment control variables in both the outcome and response models. For the US experiment, we include gender, age, age squared, education, marital status, number of years in residence, employment status, log income, and an indicator variable 12

13 about whether any of the pre-treatment covariates is missing. For the German experiment, we include gender, age, age squared, and the missing covariate indicator variable. Our inference is based on the following complete-data likelihood function, n J 1 { pj (Z i ) Y i (1 p j (Z i )) } (1 Y 1(T i) i =j) { r 1 (X i ) R i (1 r 1 (X i )) } (1 R Y i) i i=1 j=0 { r 0 (X i ) R i (1 r 0 (X i )) (1 R i) } 1 Y i, (5) from which the observed-data likelihood function can be computed by integrating out the missing data, i.e., Y i with R i = 0. Finally, we assume conjugate prior distributions with large variances for the coefficients, i.e., (α j, β, γ) N(0, 100I) and δ k N(0, 10) where I represents the identity matrix. A Markov chain Monte Carlo algorithm is constructed to sample from the posterior distributions. The algorithm is based on a standard Gibbs sampler for the probit regression, but we apply marginal data augmentation to improve its convergence (29). For each analysis, a total of one million draws are obtained and the inference is based on every 10th draw of the second half of the chain. The standard diagnostics tools indicate that a satisfactory degree of convergence is attained. References and Notes 1. A. Ellis, M. Gratschew, J. H. Pammett, E. Thiessen, Engaging the Electorate: Initiatives to Promote Voter Turnout From Around the World (International Institute for Democracy and Electoral Assistance, Stockholm, 2006). 2. R. L. Pintor, M. Gratschew, Voter turnout since 1945 : a global report (International Institute for Democracy and Electoral Assistance, Stockholm, 2002). 13

14 3. United States Election Assistance Commission, A Summary of the 2004 Election Day Survey. How We Voted: People, Ballots, and Polling Places (United States Election Assistance Commission, New York, 2005). 4. National Annenberg Election Survey, Early voting reaches record levels in 2004, national annenberg election survey shows, Press release, Annenberg Public Policy Center of the University of Pennsylvania (March 24, 2005). 5. J. Dao, New York Times pp. 7 November, sec.a (2000,). 6. A. Blais, To Vote or not to Vote: The Merits and Limits of Rational Choice Theory (University of Pittsburgh Press, Pittsburgh, 2000). 7. P. E. Converse, Non-voting among young adults in the united states, In Political parties and political behavior, W. J. Crotty, D. M. Freeman, D. S. Gatlin, eds. (Allyn & Bacon, Boston, 1971). 8. D. E. Sprott, et al., Social Influence 1, 128 (2006). 9. V. G. Morwitz, E. J. Johnson, D. Schmittlein, Journal of Consumer Research 20, 46 (1993). 10. S. J. Sherman, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39, 211 (1980). 11. V. G. Morwitz, G. J. Fitzsimons, Journal of Consumer Psychology pp (2004). 12. H. F. Gosnell, Getting-Out-the-Vote: An experiment in the stimulation of voting (University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1927). 13. C. B. Mann, The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 601, 155 (2005). 14

15 14. A. F. Greenwald, C. Carnot, R. Beach, B. Young, Journal of Applied Psychology 72, 315 (1987). 15. K. Imai, American Political Science Review 99, 283 (2005). 16. P. M. Gollwitzer, P. Sheeran, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 38, 69 (2006). 17. U. M. Dholakia, R. P. Bagozzi, Journal of Applied Social Psychology 33, 889 (2003). 18. Y. Horiuchi, K. Imai, N. Taniguchi, American Journal of Political Science (2007). Forthcoming. 19. B. C. Burden, Political Analysis 8, 389 (2000). 20. K. Imai, Statistical analysis of randomized experiments with nonignorable missing binary outcomes, Tech. rep., Department of Politics, Princeton University (2006). 21. E. J. Johnson, D. G. Goldstein, Science 302, 1338 (2003). 22. B. C. Madrian, D. F. Shea, The Quarterly Journal of Economics 116, 1149 (2001). 23. R. Greevy, B. Lu, J. H. Silber, P. Rosenbaum, Biostatistics 5, 263 (2004). 24. D. Granberg, S. Holmberg, American Journal of Political Science 35, 448 (1991). 25. R. M. Alvarez, S. Ansolabehere, E. Antonsson, J. Bruck, Voting: What Is, What Could Be (CalTech/MIT Voting Technology Project, 2001). 26. A. Göritz, Using online panels in psychological research, In Oxford Handbook of Internet Psychology, A. Joinson, K. McKenna, T. Postmes, U. D. Reips, eds. (Oxford University Press, Oxford, in press). 27. P. W. Holland, Journal of the American Statistical Association 81, 945 (1986). 15

16 28. K. Hirano, G. Imbens, G. Ridder, D. B. Rubin, Econometrica 69, 1645 (2001). 29. K. Imai, D. A. van Dyk, Journal of Econometrics 124, 311 (2005). 30. Imai thanks the US National Science Foundation (SES ) and Princeton University Committee on Research in the Humanities and Social Sciences for financial support. Goldstein thanks Konstanze Albrecht for research assistance. This work was supported by a University of Erlangen-Nuremberg postdoctoral scholarship (HWP) to Göritz. 16

17 Average Treatment Effects (Turnout Probability) US Election Day Voting US Early Voting Mere measurement vs. Control Germany Election Day Voting US Election Day Voting US Early Voting Implementation intentions vs. Control Germany Election Day Voting Figure 1: Estimated Sample Average Treatment Effects of Mere Measurement and Implementation Intentions from the US and German Election Experiments. The figure shows 50% (thick bars) and 95% (thin lines) Bayesian confidence intervals as well as point estimates. For the US experiment, in which treatments came two months before the election, mere measurement does not increase the probability of in-person voting (the estimated average treatment effect or ATE is and Pr(ATE > 0) = 0.38), while it has a modest positive effect on the probability of early voting (ATE is with Pr(ATE > 0) = 0.85). The implementation intentions treatment increases the probability of both in-person voting (ATE is and Pr(ATE > 0) = 0.93) and early voting (ATE is and Pr(ATE > 0) = 0.97). For the German experiment, in which treatments immediately preceded the election, both mere measurement and implementation intentions increase the probability of in-person voting (ATE = and Pr(ATE > 0) = 0.95 for mere measurement; ATE = with Pr(ATE > 0) = 0.89 for implementation intentions). 17

Case Study: Get out the Vote

Case Study: Get out the Vote Case Study: Get out the Vote Do Phone Calls to Encourage Voting Work? Why Randomize? This case study is based on Comparing Experimental and Matching Methods Using a Large-Scale Field Experiment on Voter

More information

Online Appendix for Redistricting and the Causal Impact of Race on Voter Turnout

Online Appendix for Redistricting and the Causal Impact of Race on Voter Turnout Online Appendix for Redistricting and the Causal Impact of Race on Voter Turnout Bernard L. Fraga Contents Appendix A Details of Estimation Strategy 1 A.1 Hypotheses.....................................

More information

1. The Relationship Between Party Control, Latino CVAP and the Passage of Bills Benefitting Immigrants

1. The Relationship Between Party Control, Latino CVAP and the Passage of Bills Benefitting Immigrants The Ideological and Electoral Determinants of Laws Targeting Undocumented Migrants in the U.S. States Online Appendix In this additional methodological appendix I present some alternative model specifications

More information

Statistical Analysis of Endorsement Experiments: Measuring Support for Militant Groups in Pakistan

Statistical Analysis of Endorsement Experiments: Measuring Support for Militant Groups in Pakistan Statistical Analysis of Endorsement Experiments: Measuring Support for Militant Groups in Pakistan Kosuke Imai Department of Politics Princeton University Joint work with Will Bullock and Jacob Shapiro

More information

The Effect of Ballot Order: Evidence from the Spanish Senate

The Effect of Ballot Order: Evidence from the Spanish Senate The Effect of Ballot Order: Evidence from the Spanish Senate Manuel Bagues Berta Esteve-Volart November 20, 2011 PRELIMINARY AND INCOMPLETE Abstract This paper analyzes the relevance of ballot order in

More information

1. A Republican edge in terms of self-described interest in the election. 2. Lower levels of self-described interest among younger and Latino

1. A Republican edge in terms of self-described interest in the election. 2. Lower levels of self-described interest among younger and Latino 2 Academics use political polling as a measure about the viability of survey research can it accurately predict the result of a national election? The answer continues to be yes. There is compelling evidence

More information

VoteCastr methodology

VoteCastr methodology VoteCastr methodology Introduction Going into Election Day, we will have a fairly good idea of which candidate would win each state if everyone voted. However, not everyone votes. The levels of enthusiasm

More information

Online Appendix 1: Treatment Stimuli

Online Appendix 1: Treatment Stimuli Online Appendix 1: Treatment Stimuli Polarized Stimulus: 1 Electorate as Divided as Ever by Jefferson Graham (USA Today) In the aftermath of the 2012 presidential election, interviews with voters at a

More information

Experiments in Election Reform: Voter Perceptions of Campaigns Under Preferential and Plurality Voting

Experiments in Election Reform: Voter Perceptions of Campaigns Under Preferential and Plurality Voting Experiments in Election Reform: Voter Perceptions of Campaigns Under Preferential and Plurality Voting Caroline Tolbert, University of Iowa (caroline-tolbert@uiowa.edu) Collaborators: Todd Donovan, Western

More information

On the Causes and Consequences of Ballot Order Effects

On the Causes and Consequences of Ballot Order Effects Polit Behav (2013) 35:175 197 DOI 10.1007/s11109-011-9189-2 ORIGINAL PAPER On the Causes and Consequences of Ballot Order Effects Marc Meredith Yuval Salant Published online: 6 January 2012 Ó Springer

More information

American public has much to learn about presidential candidates issue positions, National Annenberg Election Survey shows

American public has much to learn about presidential candidates issue positions, National Annenberg Election Survey shows For Immediate Release: September 26, 2008 For more information: Kate Kenski, kkenski@email.arizona.edu Kathleen Hall Jamieson, kjamieson@asc.upenn.edu Visit: www.annenbergpublicpolicycenter.org American

More information

Learning from Small Subsamples without Cherry Picking: The Case of Non-Citizen Registration and Voting

Learning from Small Subsamples without Cherry Picking: The Case of Non-Citizen Registration and Voting Learning from Small Subsamples without Cherry Picking: The Case of Non-Citizen Registration and Voting Jesse Richman Old Dominion University jrichman@odu.edu David C. Earnest Old Dominion University, and

More information

Methodology. 1 State benchmarks are from the American Community Survey Three Year averages

Methodology. 1 State benchmarks are from the American Community Survey Three Year averages The Choice is Yours Comparing Alternative Likely Voter Models within Probability and Non-Probability Samples By Robert Benford, Randall K Thomas, Jennifer Agiesta, Emily Swanson Likely voter models often

More information

Supporting Information for Do Perceptions of Ballot Secrecy Influence Turnout? Results from a Field Experiment

Supporting Information for Do Perceptions of Ballot Secrecy Influence Turnout? Results from a Field Experiment Supporting Information for Do Perceptions of Ballot Secrecy Influence Turnout? Results from a Field Experiment Alan S. Gerber Yale University Professor Department of Political Science Institution for Social

More information

Gender preference and age at arrival among Asian immigrant women to the US

Gender preference and age at arrival among Asian immigrant women to the US Gender preference and age at arrival among Asian immigrant women to the US Ben Ost a and Eva Dziadula b a Department of Economics, University of Illinois at Chicago, 601 South Morgan UH718 M/C144 Chicago,

More information

Congruence in Political Parties

Congruence in Political Parties Descriptive Representation of Women and Ideological Congruence in Political Parties Georgia Kernell Northwestern University gkernell@northwestern.edu June 15, 2011 Abstract This paper examines the relationship

More information

The Cook Political Report / LSU Manship School Midterm Election Poll

The Cook Political Report / LSU Manship School Midterm Election Poll The Cook Political Report / LSU Manship School Midterm Election Poll The Cook Political Report-LSU Manship School poll, a national survey with an oversample of voters in the most competitive U.S. House

More information

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF UNDERGRADUATE RESEARCH VOL. 3 NO. 4 (2005)

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF UNDERGRADUATE RESEARCH VOL. 3 NO. 4 (2005) , Partisanship and the Post Bounce: A MemoryBased Model of Post Presidential Candidate Evaluations Part II Empirical Results Justin Grimmer Department of Mathematics and Computer Science Wabash College

More information

Colorado 2014: Comparisons of Predicted and Actual Turnout

Colorado 2014: Comparisons of Predicted and Actual Turnout Colorado 2014: Comparisons of Predicted and Actual Turnout Date 2017-08-28 Project name Colorado 2014 Voter File Analysis Prepared for Washington Monthly and Project Partners Prepared by Pantheon Analytics

More information

Following the Leader: The Impact of Presidential Campaign Visits on Legislative Support for the President's Policy Preferences

Following the Leader: The Impact of Presidential Campaign Visits on Legislative Support for the President's Policy Preferences University of Colorado, Boulder CU Scholar Undergraduate Honors Theses Honors Program Spring 2011 Following the Leader: The Impact of Presidential Campaign Visits on Legislative Support for the President's

More information

Ohio State University

Ohio State University Fake News Did Have a Significant Impact on the Vote in the 2016 Election: Original Full-Length Version with Methodological Appendix By Richard Gunther, Paul A. Beck, and Erik C. Nisbet Ohio State University

More information

WP 2015: 9. Education and electoral participation: Reported versus actual voting behaviour. Ivar Kolstad and Arne Wiig VOTE

WP 2015: 9. Education and electoral participation: Reported versus actual voting behaviour. Ivar Kolstad and Arne Wiig VOTE WP 2015: 9 Reported versus actual voting behaviour Ivar Kolstad and Arne Wiig VOTE Chr. Michelsen Institute (CMI) is an independent, non-profit research institution and a major international centre in

More information

14.11: Experiments in Political Science

14.11: Experiments in Political Science 14.11: Experiments in Political Science Prof. Esther Duflo May 9, 2006 Voting is a paradoxical behavior: the chance of being the pivotal voter in an election is close to zero, and yet people do vote...

More information

Chapter Four: Chamber Competitiveness, Political Polarization, and Political Parties

Chapter Four: Chamber Competitiveness, Political Polarization, and Political Parties Chapter Four: Chamber Competitiveness, Political Polarization, and Political Parties Building off of the previous chapter in this dissertation, this chapter investigates the involvement of political parties

More information

Who Votes Without Identification? Using Affidavits from Michigan to Learn About the Potential Impact of Strict Photo Voter Identification Laws

Who Votes Without Identification? Using Affidavits from Michigan to Learn About the Potential Impact of Strict Photo Voter Identification Laws Using Affidavits from Michigan to Learn About the Potential Impact of Strict Photo Voter Identification Laws Phoebe Henninger Marc Meredith Michael Morse University of Michigan University of Pennsylvania

More information

CALTECH/MIT VOTING TECHNOLOGY PROJECT A

CALTECH/MIT VOTING TECHNOLOGY PROJECT A CALTECH/MIT VOTING TECHNOLOGY PROJECT A multi-disciplinary, collaborative project of the California Institute of Technology Pasadena, California 91125 and the Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge,

More information

Supplementary Materials for Strategic Abstention in Proportional Representation Systems (Evidence from Multiple Countries)

Supplementary Materials for Strategic Abstention in Proportional Representation Systems (Evidence from Multiple Countries) Supplementary Materials for Strategic Abstention in Proportional Representation Systems (Evidence from Multiple Countries) Guillem Riambau July 15, 2018 1 1 Construction of variables and descriptive statistics.

More information

California Ballot Reform Panel Survey Page 1

California Ballot Reform Panel Survey Page 1 CALIFORNIA BALLOT RE FORM PANEL SURVEY 2011-2012 Interview Dates: Wave One: June 14-July 1, 2011 Wave Two: December 15-January 2, 2012 Sample size Wave One: (N=1555) Wave Two: (N=1064) Margin of error

More information

Supporting Information Political Quid Pro Quo Agreements: An Experimental Study

Supporting Information Political Quid Pro Quo Agreements: An Experimental Study Supporting Information Political Quid Pro Quo Agreements: An Experimental Study Jens Großer Florida State University and IAS, Princeton Ernesto Reuben Columbia University and IZA Agnieszka Tymula New York

More information

Women as Policy Makers: Evidence from a Randomized Policy Experiment in India

Women as Policy Makers: Evidence from a Randomized Policy Experiment in India Women as Policy Makers: Evidence from a Randomized Policy Experiment in India Chattopadhayay and Duflo (Econometrica 2004) Presented by Nicolas Guida Johnson and Ngoc Nguyen Nov 8, 2018 Introduction Research

More information

Practice Questions for Exam #2

Practice Questions for Exam #2 Fall 2007 Page 1 Practice Questions for Exam #2 1. Suppose that we have collected a stratified random sample of 1,000 Hispanic adults and 1,000 non-hispanic adults. These respondents are asked whether

More information

What is The Probability Your Vote will Make a Difference?

What is The Probability Your Vote will Make a Difference? Berkeley Law From the SelectedWorks of Aaron Edlin 2009 What is The Probability Your Vote will Make a Difference? Andrew Gelman, Columbia University Nate Silver Aaron S. Edlin, University of California,

More information

Personnel Politics: Elections, Clientelistic Competition, and Teacher Hiring in Indonesia

Personnel Politics: Elections, Clientelistic Competition, and Teacher Hiring in Indonesia Personnel Politics: Elections, Clientelistic Competition, and Teacher Hiring in Indonesia Jan H. Pierskalla and Audrey Sacks Department of Political Science, The Ohio State University GPSURR, World Bank

More information

Estimating Neighborhood Effects on Turnout from Geocoded Voter Registration Records

Estimating Neighborhood Effects on Turnout from Geocoded Voter Registration Records Estimating Neighborhood Effects on Turnout from Geocoded Voter Registration Records Michael Barber Kosuke Imai First Draft: April, 13 This Draft: January 8, 1 Abstract Do voters turn out more or less frequently

More information

Research Statement. Jeffrey J. Harden. 2 Dissertation Research: The Dimensions of Representation

Research Statement. Jeffrey J. Harden. 2 Dissertation Research: The Dimensions of Representation Research Statement Jeffrey J. Harden 1 Introduction My research agenda includes work in both quantitative methodology and American politics. In methodology I am broadly interested in developing and evaluating

More information

FOR RELEASE APRIL 26, 2018

FOR RELEASE APRIL 26, 2018 FOR RELEASE APRIL 26, 2018 FOR MEDIA OR OTHER INQUIRIES: Carroll Doherty, Director of Political Research Jocelyn Kiley, Associate Director, Research Bridget Johnson, Communications Associate 202.419.4372

More information

Immigrant Legalization

Immigrant Legalization Technical Appendices Immigrant Legalization Assessing the Labor Market Effects Laura Hill Magnus Lofstrom Joseph Hayes Contents Appendix A. Data from the 2003 New Immigrant Survey Appendix B. Measuring

More information

Biases in Message Credibility and Voter Expectations EGAP Preregisration GATED until June 28, 2017 Summary.

Biases in Message Credibility and Voter Expectations EGAP Preregisration GATED until June 28, 2017 Summary. Biases in Message Credibility and Voter Expectations EGAP Preregisration GATED until June 28, 2017 Summary. Election polls in horserace coverage characterize a competitive information environment with

More information

Lab 3: Logistic regression models

Lab 3: Logistic regression models Lab 3: Logistic regression models In this lab, we will apply logistic regression models to United States (US) presidential election data sets. The main purpose is to predict the outcomes of presidential

More information

Honors General Exam Part 1: Microeconomics (33 points) Harvard University

Honors General Exam Part 1: Microeconomics (33 points) Harvard University Honors General Exam Part 1: Microeconomics (33 points) Harvard University April 9, 2014 QUESTION 1. (6 points) The inverse demand function for apples is defined by the equation p = 214 5q, where q is the

More information

IS THE MEASURED BLACK-WHITE WAGE GAP AMONG WOMEN TOO SMALL? Derek Neal University of Wisconsin Presented Nov 6, 2000 PRELIMINARY

IS THE MEASURED BLACK-WHITE WAGE GAP AMONG WOMEN TOO SMALL? Derek Neal University of Wisconsin Presented Nov 6, 2000 PRELIMINARY IS THE MEASURED BLACK-WHITE WAGE GAP AMONG WOMEN TOO SMALL? Derek Neal University of Wisconsin Presented Nov 6, 2000 PRELIMINARY Over twenty years ago, Butler and Heckman (1977) raised the possibility

More information

Migration and Tourism Flows to New Zealand

Migration and Tourism Flows to New Zealand Migration and Tourism Flows to New Zealand Murat Genç University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand Email address for correspondence: murat.genc@otago.ac.nz 30 April 2010 PRELIMINARY WORK IN PROGRESS NOT FOR

More information

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Materials Supplementary Materials October 10, 201 1 Ballot Language The exact language on the ballot in Milwaukee was as follows: Shall the City of Milwaukee adopt Common Council File 080420, being a substitute

More information

POLL: CLINTON MAINTAINS BIG LEAD OVER TRUMP IN BAY STATE. As early voting nears, Democrat holds 32-point advantage in presidential race

POLL: CLINTON MAINTAINS BIG LEAD OVER TRUMP IN BAY STATE. As early voting nears, Democrat holds 32-point advantage in presidential race DATE: Oct. 6, FOR FURTHER INFORMATION, CONTACT: Brian Zelasko at 413-796-2261 (office) or 413 297-8237 (cell) David Stawasz at 413-796-2026 (office) or 413-214-8001 (cell) POLL: CLINTON MAINTAINS BIG LEAD

More information

College Voting in the 2018 Midterms: A Survey of US College Students. (Medium)

College Voting in the 2018 Midterms: A Survey of US College Students. (Medium) College Voting in the 2018 Midterms: A Survey of US College Students (Medium) 1 Overview: An online survey of 3,633 current college students was conducted using College Reaction s national polling infrastructure

More information

Delia Bailey. Center for Empirical Research in the Law Washington University Campus Box 1120 One Brookings Drive St.

Delia Bailey. Center for Empirical Research in the Law Washington University Campus Box 1120 One Brookings Drive St. Delia Bailey Center for Empirical Research in the Law Washington University Campus Box 1120 One Brookings Drive St. Louis, MO 63130 Phone: (314) 935-9640 Email: dbailey@wustl.edu Web: http://delia.wustl.edu

More information

Estimating Incumbency Advantage: Evidence from Three Natural Experiments *

Estimating Incumbency Advantage: Evidence from Three Natural Experiments * . Estimating Incumbency Advantage: Evidence from Three Natural Experiments * Yusaku Horiuchi Crawford School of Economics and Government Australian National Universy, ACT 0200, Australia yusaku.horiuchi@anu.edu.au

More information

CONTACT: TIM VERCELLOTTI, Ph.D., (732) , EXT. 285; (919) (cell) CRANKY ELECTORATE STILL GIVES DEMOCRATS THE EDGE

CONTACT: TIM VERCELLOTTI, Ph.D., (732) , EXT. 285; (919) (cell) CRANKY ELECTORATE STILL GIVES DEMOCRATS THE EDGE - Eagleton Poll EMBARGOED UNTIL 9 A.M. EDT OCT. 25, 2007 Oct. 25, 2007 (Release 163-1) CONTACT: TIM VERCELLOTTI, Ph.D., (732) 932-9384, EXT. 285; (919) 812-3452 (cell) CRANKY ELECTORATE STILL GIVES DEMOCRATS

More information

The RAND 2016 Presidential Election Panel Survey (PEPS) Michael Pollard, Joshua Mendelsohn, Alerk Amin

The RAND 2016 Presidential Election Panel Survey (PEPS) Michael Pollard, Joshua Mendelsohn, Alerk Amin The RAND 2016 Presidential Election Panel Survey (PEPS) Michael Pollard, Joshua Mendelsohn, Alerk Amin mpollard@rand.org May 14, 2016 Six surveys throughout election season Comprehensive baseline in December

More information

Corruption, Political Instability and Firm-Level Export Decisions. Kul Kapri 1 Rowan University. August 2018

Corruption, Political Instability and Firm-Level Export Decisions. Kul Kapri 1 Rowan University. August 2018 Corruption, Political Instability and Firm-Level Export Decisions Kul Kapri 1 Rowan University August 2018 Abstract In this paper I use South Asian firm-level data to examine whether the impact of corruption

More information

Report for the Associated Press: Illinois and Georgia Election Studies in November 2014

Report for the Associated Press: Illinois and Georgia Election Studies in November 2014 Report for the Associated Press: Illinois and Georgia Election Studies in November 2014 Randall K. Thomas, Frances M. Barlas, Linda McPetrie, Annie Weber, Mansour Fahimi, & Robert Benford GfK Custom Research

More information

BLISS INSTITUTE 2006 GENERAL ELECTION SURVEY

BLISS INSTITUTE 2006 GENERAL ELECTION SURVEY BLISS INSTITUTE 2006 GENERAL ELECTION SURVEY Ray C. Bliss Institute of Applied Politics The University of Akron Executive Summary The Bliss Institute 2006 General Election Survey finds Democrat Ted Strickland

More information

Incumbency as a Source of Spillover Effects in Mixed Electoral Systems: Evidence from a Regression-Discontinuity Design.

Incumbency as a Source of Spillover Effects in Mixed Electoral Systems: Evidence from a Regression-Discontinuity Design. Incumbency as a Source of Spillover Effects in Mixed Electoral Systems: Evidence from a Regression-Discontinuity Design Forthcoming, Electoral Studies Web Supplement Jens Hainmueller Holger Lutz Kern September

More information

Do natives beliefs about refugees education level affect attitudes toward refugees? Evidence from randomized survey experiments

Do natives beliefs about refugees education level affect attitudes toward refugees? Evidence from randomized survey experiments Do natives beliefs about refugees education level affect attitudes toward refugees? Evidence from randomized survey experiments Philipp Lergetporer Marc Piopiunik Lisa Simon AEA Meeting, Philadelphia 5

More information

Remittances and the Brain Drain: Evidence from Microdata for Sub-Saharan Africa

Remittances and the Brain Drain: Evidence from Microdata for Sub-Saharan Africa Remittances and the Brain Drain: Evidence from Microdata for Sub-Saharan Africa Julia Bredtmann 1, Fernanda Martinez Flores 1,2, and Sebastian Otten 1,2,3 1 RWI, Rheinisch-Westfälisches Institut für Wirtschaftsforschung

More information

GEORG-AUGUST-UNIVERSITÄT GÖTTINGEN

GEORG-AUGUST-UNIVERSITÄT GÖTTINGEN GEORG-AUGUST-UNIVERSITÄT GÖTTINGEN FACULTY OF ECONOMIC SCIENCES CHAIR OF MACROECONOMICS AND DEVELOPMENT Bachelor Seminar Economics of the very long run: Economics of Islam Summer semester 2017 Does Secular

More information

Model of Voting. February 15, Abstract. This paper uses United States congressional district level data to identify how incumbency,

Model of Voting. February 15, Abstract. This paper uses United States congressional district level data to identify how incumbency, U.S. Congressional Vote Empirics: A Discrete Choice Model of Voting Kyle Kretschman The University of Texas Austin kyle.kretschman@mail.utexas.edu Nick Mastronardi United States Air Force Academy nickmastronardi@gmail.com

More information

Wisconsin Economic Scorecard

Wisconsin Economic Scorecard RESEARCH PAPER> May 2012 Wisconsin Economic Scorecard Analysis: Determinants of Individual Opinion about the State Economy Joseph Cera Researcher Survey Center Manager The Wisconsin Economic Scorecard

More information

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, May, 2017, Partisan Identification Is Sticky, but About 10% Switched Parties Over the Past Year

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, May, 2017, Partisan Identification Is Sticky, but About 10% Switched Parties Over the Past Year NUMBERS, FACTS AND TRENDS SHAPING THE WORLD FOR RELEASE MAY 17, 2017 FOR MEDIA OR OTHER INQUIRIES: Carroll Doherty, Director of Political Research Jocelyn Kiley, Associate Director, Research Bridget Johnson,

More information

Partisan Nation: The Rise of Affective Partisan Polarization in the American Electorate

Partisan Nation: The Rise of Affective Partisan Polarization in the American Electorate Partisan Nation: The Rise of Affective Partisan Polarization in the American Electorate Alan I. Abramowitz Department of Political Science Emory University Abstract Partisan conflict has reached new heights

More information

THE PUBLIC AND THE CRITICAL ISSUES BEFORE CONGRESS IN THE SUMMER AND FALL OF 2017

THE PUBLIC AND THE CRITICAL ISSUES BEFORE CONGRESS IN THE SUMMER AND FALL OF 2017 THE PUBLIC AND THE CRITICAL ISSUES BEFORE CONGRESS IN THE SUMMER AND FALL OF 2017 July 2017 1 INTRODUCTION At the time this poll s results are being released, the Congress is engaged in a number of debates

More information

Compulsory versus Voluntary Voting Mechanisms: An Experimental Study

Compulsory versus Voluntary Voting Mechanisms: An Experimental Study Compulsory versus Voluntary Voting Mechanisms: An Experimental Study Sourav Bhattacharya John Duffy Sun-Tak Kim January 31, 2011 Abstract This paper uses laboratory experiments to study the impact of voting

More information

CSES Module 5 Pretest Report: Greece. August 31, 2016

CSES Module 5 Pretest Report: Greece. August 31, 2016 CSES Module 5 Pretest Report: Greece August 31, 2016 1 Contents INTRODUCTION... 4 BACKGROUND... 4 METHODOLOGY... 4 Sample... 4 Representativeness... 4 DISTRIBUTIONS OF KEY VARIABLES... 7 ATTITUDES ABOUT

More information

APPENDIX TO MILITARY ALLIANCES AND PUBLIC SUPPORT FOR WAR TABLE OF CONTENTS I. YOUGOV SURVEY: QUESTIONS... 3

APPENDIX TO MILITARY ALLIANCES AND PUBLIC SUPPORT FOR WAR TABLE OF CONTENTS I. YOUGOV SURVEY: QUESTIONS... 3 APPENDIX TO MILITARY ALLIANCES AND PUBLIC SUPPORT FOR WAR TABLE OF CONTENTS I. YOUGOV SURVEY: QUESTIONS... 3 RANDOMIZED TREATMENTS... 3 TEXT OF THE EXPERIMENT... 4 ATTITUDINAL CONTROLS... 10 DEMOGRAPHIC

More information

Rick Santorum has erased 7.91 point deficit to move into a statistical tie with Mitt Romney the night before voters go to the polls in Michigan.

Rick Santorum has erased 7.91 point deficit to move into a statistical tie with Mitt Romney the night before voters go to the polls in Michigan. Rick Santorum has erased 7.91 point deficit to move into a statistical tie with Mitt Romney the night before voters go to the polls in Michigan. February 27, 2012 Contact: Eric Foster, Foster McCollum

More information

Michigan 14th Congressional District Democratic Primary Election Exclusive Polling Study for Fox 2 News Detroit.

Michigan 14th Congressional District Democratic Primary Election Exclusive Polling Study for Fox 2 News Detroit. Michigan 14th Congressional District Democratic Primary Election Exclusive Polling Study for Fox 2 News Detroit. Automated Poll Methodology and Statistics Aggregate Results Conducted by Foster McCollum

More information

Study Background. Part I. Voter Experience with Ballots, Precincts, and Poll Workers

Study Background. Part I. Voter Experience with Ballots, Precincts, and Poll Workers The 2006 New Mexico First Congressional District Registered Voter Election Administration Report Study Background August 11, 2007 Lonna Rae Atkeson University of New Mexico In 2006, the University of New

More information

ANES Panel Study Proposal Voter Turnout and the Electoral College 1. Voter Turnout and Electoral College Attitudes. Gregory D.

ANES Panel Study Proposal Voter Turnout and the Electoral College 1. Voter Turnout and Electoral College Attitudes. Gregory D. ANES Panel Study Proposal Voter Turnout and the Electoral College 1 Voter Turnout and Electoral College Attitudes Gregory D. Webster University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Keywords: Voter turnout;

More information

Objectives and Context

Objectives and Context Encouraging Ballot Return via Text Message: Portland Community College Bond Election 2017 Prepared by Christopher B. Mann, Ph.D. with Alexis Cantor and Isabelle Fischer Executive Summary A series of text

More information

Voting and Electoral Competition

Voting and Electoral Competition Voting and Electoral Competition Prof. Panu Poutvaara University of Munich and Ifo Institute On the organization of the course Lectures, exam at the end Articles to read. In more technical articles, it

More information

Proposal for the 2016 ANES Time Series. Quantitative Predictions of State and National Election Outcomes

Proposal for the 2016 ANES Time Series. Quantitative Predictions of State and National Election Outcomes Proposal for the 2016 ANES Time Series Quantitative Predictions of State and National Election Outcomes Keywords: Election predictions, motivated reasoning, natural experiments, citizen competence, measurement

More information

Turnout and Strength of Habits

Turnout and Strength of Habits Turnout and Strength of Habits John H. Aldrich Wendy Wood Jacob M. Montgomery Duke University I) Introduction Social scientists are much better at explaining for whom people vote than whether people vote

More information

FOR RELEASE AUGUST 16, 2018

FOR RELEASE AUGUST 16, 2018 FOR RELEASE AUGUST 16, 2018 FOR MEDIA OR OTHER INQUIRIES: Carroll Doherty, Director of Political Research Jocelyn Kiley, Associate Director, Research Bridget Johnson, Communications Manager 202.419.4372

More information

Are Polls Good for the Voter? On the Impact of Attitudes Towards Surveys in Electoral Campaigns

Are Polls Good for the Voter? On the Impact of Attitudes Towards Surveys in Electoral Campaigns Are Polls Good for the Voter? On the Impact of Attitudes Towards Surveys in Electoral Campaigns Paper presented by Claire Durand, Universite de Montreal John Goyder, University of Waterloo ISA Research

More information

De Facto Disenfranchisement: Estimating the Impact of Voting Rights Information on Ex- Felon Attitudes towards Voting and Civic Engagement

De Facto Disenfranchisement: Estimating the Impact of Voting Rights Information on Ex- Felon Attitudes towards Voting and Civic Engagement De Facto Disenfranchisement: Estimating the Impact of Voting Rights Information on Ex- Felon Attitudes towards Voting and Civic Engagement David S. McCahon University of California, Riverside Department

More information

Supplementary Materials for

Supplementary Materials for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/science.aag2147/dc1 Supplementary Materials for How economic, humanitarian, and religious concerns shape European attitudes toward asylum seekers This PDF file includes

More information

Robert H. Prisuta, American Association of Retired Persons (AARP) 601 E Street, N.W., Washington, D.C

Robert H. Prisuta, American Association of Retired Persons (AARP) 601 E Street, N.W., Washington, D.C A POST-ELECTION BANDWAGON EFFECT? COMPARING NATIONAL EXIT POLL DATA WITH A GENERAL POPULATION SURVEY Robert H. Prisuta, American Association of Retired Persons (AARP) 601 E Street, N.W., Washington, D.C.

More information

Appendices for Elections and the Regression-Discontinuity Design: Lessons from Close U.S. House Races,

Appendices for Elections and the Regression-Discontinuity Design: Lessons from Close U.S. House Races, Appendices for Elections and the Regression-Discontinuity Design: Lessons from Close U.S. House Races, 1942 2008 Devin M. Caughey Jasjeet S. Sekhon 7/20/2011 (10:34) Ph.D. candidate, Travers Department

More information

Business Practice Group Report for the 2014 General Election

Business Practice Group Report for the 2014 General Election Business Practice Group Report for the 2014 General Election The following is an executive summary of two surveys conducted by the Business Practice Group (BPG), testimonials from Clerk and Recorder s

More information

AmericasBarometer Insights: 2011 Number 63

AmericasBarometer Insights: 2011 Number 63 AmericasBarometer Insights: 2011 Number 63 Compulsory Voting and the Decision to Vote By arturo.maldonado@vanderbilt.edu Vanderbilt University Executive Summary. Does compulsory voting alter the rational

More information

Ipsos Poll Conducted for Reuters Daily Election Tracking:

Ipsos Poll Conducted for Reuters Daily Election Tracking: : 11.05.12 These are findings from an Ipsos poll conducted for Thomson Reuters from Nov. 1.-5, 2012. For the survey, a sample of 5,643 American registered voters and 4,725 Likely Voters (all age 18 and

More information

The Partisan Effects of Voter Turnout

The Partisan Effects of Voter Turnout The Partisan Effects of Voter Turnout Alexander Kendall March 29, 2004 1 The Problem According to the Washington Post, Republicans are urged to pray for poor weather on national election days, so that

More information

The California Primary and Redistricting

The California Primary and Redistricting The California Primary and Redistricting This study analyzes what is the important impact of changes in the primary voting rules after a Congressional and Legislative Redistricting. Under a citizen s committee,

More information

State of the Facts 2018

State of the Facts 2018 State of the Facts 2018 Part 2 of 2 Summary of Results September 2018 Objective and Methodology USAFacts conducted the second annual State of the Facts survey in 2018 to revisit questions asked in 2017

More information

DATA ANALYSIS USING SETUPS AND SPSS: AMERICAN VOTING BEHAVIOR IN PRESIDENTIAL ELECTIONS

DATA ANALYSIS USING SETUPS AND SPSS: AMERICAN VOTING BEHAVIOR IN PRESIDENTIAL ELECTIONS Poli 300 Handout B N. R. Miller DATA ANALYSIS USING SETUPS AND SPSS: AMERICAN VOTING BEHAVIOR IN IDENTIAL ELECTIONS 1972-2004 The original SETUPS: AMERICAN VOTING BEHAVIOR IN IDENTIAL ELECTIONS 1972-1992

More information

An Empirical Analysis of Pakistan s Bilateral Trade: A Gravity Model Approach

An Empirical Analysis of Pakistan s Bilateral Trade: A Gravity Model Approach 103 An Empirical Analysis of Pakistan s Bilateral Trade: A Gravity Model Approach Shaista Khan 1 Ihtisham ul Haq 2 Dilawar Khan 3 This study aimed to investigate Pakistan s bilateral trade flows with major

More information

Elite Polarization and Mass Political Engagement: Information, Alienation, and Mobilization

Elite Polarization and Mass Political Engagement: Information, Alienation, and Mobilization JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL AND AREA STUDIES Volume 20, Number 1, 2013, pp.89-109 89 Elite Polarization and Mass Political Engagement: Information, Alienation, and Mobilization Jae Mook Lee Using the cumulative

More information

Chapter 6 Online Appendix. general these issues do not cause significant problems for our analysis in this chapter. One

Chapter 6 Online Appendix. general these issues do not cause significant problems for our analysis in this chapter. One Chapter 6 Online Appendix Potential shortcomings of SF-ratio analysis Using SF-ratios to understand strategic behavior is not without potential problems, but in general these issues do not cause significant

More information

Citizenship in 21 st Century America

Citizenship in 21 st Century America Citizenship in 21 st Century America T he United States is a representative democracy, in which government officials are selected by the people they represent. However, many have questioned whether democracy

More information

Extended abstract. 1. Introduction

Extended abstract. 1. Introduction Extended abstract Gender wage inequality among internal migrants: Evidence from India Ajay Sharma 1 and Mousumi Das 2 Email (corresponding author): ajays@iimidr.ac.in 1. Introduction Understanding the

More information

The National Citizen Survey

The National Citizen Survey CITY OF SARASOTA, FLORIDA 2008 3005 30th Street 777 North Capitol Street NE, Suite 500 Boulder, CO 80301 Washington, DC 20002 ww.n-r-c.com 303-444-7863 www.icma.org 202-289-ICMA P U B L I C S A F E T Y

More information

USING MULTI-MEMBER-DISTRICT ELECTIONS TO ESTIMATE THE SOURCES OF THE INCUMBENCY ADVANTAGE 1

USING MULTI-MEMBER-DISTRICT ELECTIONS TO ESTIMATE THE SOURCES OF THE INCUMBENCY ADVANTAGE 1 USING MULTI-MEMBER-DISTRICT ELECTIONS TO ESTIMATE THE SOURCES OF THE INCUMBENCY ADVANTAGE 1 Shigeo Hirano Department of Political Science Columbia University James M. Snyder, Jr. Departments of Political

More information

GW POLITICS POLL 2018 MIDTERM ELECTION WAVE 1

GW POLITICS POLL 2018 MIDTERM ELECTION WAVE 1 GW POLITICS POLL 2018 MIDTERM ELECTION WAVE 1 The survey was fielded May 14 30, 2018 with a sample of registered voters. The survey was fielded by YouGov with a sample of registered voters. YouGov recruits

More information

Iowa Voting Series, Paper 4: An Examination of Iowa Turnout Statistics Since 2000 by Party and Age Group

Iowa Voting Series, Paper 4: An Examination of Iowa Turnout Statistics Since 2000 by Party and Age Group Department of Political Science Publications 3-1-2014 Iowa Voting Series, Paper 4: An Examination of Iowa Turnout Statistics Since 2000 by Party and Age Group Timothy M. Hagle University of Iowa 2014 Timothy

More information

Being a Good Samaritan or just a politician? Empirical evidence of disaster assistance. Jeroen Klomp

Being a Good Samaritan or just a politician? Empirical evidence of disaster assistance. Jeroen Klomp Being a Good Samaritan or just a politician? Empirical evidence of disaster assistance Jeroen Klomp Netherlands Defence Academy & Wageningen University and Research The Netherlands Introduction Since 1970

More information

Capturing the Effects of Public Opinion Polls on Voter Support in the NY 25th Congressional Election

Capturing the Effects of Public Opinion Polls on Voter Support in the NY 25th Congressional Election Rochester Institute of Technology RIT Scholar Works Theses Thesis/Dissertation Collections 12-23-2014 Capturing the Effects of Public Opinion Polls on Voter Support in the NY 25th Congressional Election

More information

Determinants and Effects of Negative Advertising in Politics

Determinants and Effects of Negative Advertising in Politics Department of Economics- FEA/USP Determinants and Effects of Negative Advertising in Politics DANILO P. SOUZA MARCOS Y. NAKAGUMA WORKING PAPER SERIES Nº 2017-25 DEPARTMENT OF ECONOMICS, FEA-USP WORKING

More information

PENNSYLVANIA: SMALL LEAD FOR SACCONE IN CD18

PENNSYLVANIA: SMALL LEAD FOR SACCONE IN CD18 Please attribute this information to: Monmouth University Poll West Long Branch, NJ 07764 www.monmouth.edu/polling Follow on Twitter: @MonmouthPoll Released: Thursday, 15, Contact: PATRICK MURRAY 732-979-6769

More information

Flash Eurobarometer 431. Summary. Electoral Rights

Flash Eurobarometer 431. Summary. Electoral Rights Survey requested by the European Commission, Directorate-General for Justice and Consumers and co-ordinated by the Directorate-General for Communication This document does not represent the point of view

More information

Ipsos Poll Conducted for Reuters Daily Election Tracking:

Ipsos Poll Conducted for Reuters Daily Election Tracking: : 11.01.12 These are findings from an Ipsos poll conducted for Thomson Reuters from Oct. 28-Nov. 1, 2012. For the survey, a sample of 5,575 American registered voters and 4,556 Likely Voters (all age 18

More information