WP 2015: 9. Education and electoral participation: Reported versus actual voting behaviour. Ivar Kolstad and Arne Wiig VOTE

Similar documents
ONLINE APPENDIX: DELIBERATE DISENGAGEMENT: HOW EDUCATION

Online Appendix: The Effect of Education on Civic and Political Engagement in Non-Consolidated Democracies: Evidence from Nigeria

Comparative corporate strategies: What determines Chinese outward FDI?

The gender gap in African political participation:

Is Corruption Anti Labor?

Incumbency as a Source of Spillover Effects in Mixed Electoral Systems: Evidence from a Regression-Discontinuity Design.

English Deficiency and the Native-Immigrant Wage Gap

CMIWORKINGPAPER. What Determines Chinese Outward FDI? Ivar Kolstad Arne Wiig WP 2009: 3

The transition of corruption: From poverty to honesty

Pathbreakers? Women's Electoral Success and Future Political Participation

The Determinants of Low-Intensity Intergroup Violence: The Case of Northern Ireland. Online Appendix

Does Paternity Leave Matter for Female Employment in Developing Economies?

Media and Political Persuasion: Evidence from Russia

Remittances and the Brain Drain: Evidence from Microdata for Sub-Saharan Africa

Appendix for Citizen Preferences and Public Goods: Comparing. Preferences for Foreign Aid and Government Programs in Uganda

Supplementary information for the article:

Supporting Information for Inclusion and Public. Policy: Evidence from Sweden s Introduction of. Noncitizen Suffrage

Benefit levels and US immigrants welfare receipts

Do Migrants Improve Governance at Home? Evidence from a Voting Experiment

Supplementary Materials for Strategic Abstention in Proportional Representation Systems (Evidence from Multiple Countries)

Decentralized Despotism: How Indirect Colonial Rule Undermines Contemporary Democratic Attitudes

Self-employed immigrants and their employees: Evidence from Swedish employer-employee data

Gender preference and age at arrival among Asian immigrant women to the US

Determinants of Return Migration to Mexico Among Mexicans in the United States

SIERRA LEONE 2012 ELECTIONS PROJECT PRE-ANALYSIS PLAN: INDIVIDUAL LEVEL INTERVENTIONS

Is Information Power?

The gender gap in African political participation: Individual and contextual determinants

One in a Million: A Field Experiment on Belief Formation and Pivotal Voting

Wisconsin Economic Scorecard

Online Appendix: Unified Language, Labor and Ideology

English Deficiency and the Native-Immigrant Wage Gap in the UK

Volume 35, Issue 1. An examination of the effect of immigration on income inequality: A Gini index approach

Volume 36, Issue 1. Impact of remittances on poverty: an analysis of data from a set of developing countries

1. The Relationship Between Party Control, Latino CVAP and the Passage of Bills Benefitting Immigrants

These are the findings from the latest statewide Field Poll completed among 1,003 registered voters in early January.

Immigrant Legalization

What Can We Learn about Financial Access from U.S. Immigrants?

Surviving Elections: Election Violence, Incumbent Victory, and Post-Election Repercussions January 11, 2016

Can Politicians Police Themselves? Natural Experimental Evidence from Brazil s Audit Courts Supplementary Appendix

Online Appendix: Who Gets a Swiss Passport? A Natural Experiment in Immigrant Discrimination

Access to agricultural land, youth migration and livelihoods in Tanzania

Supplementary Materials A: Figures for All 7 Surveys Figure S1-A: Distribution of Predicted Probabilities of Voting in Primary Elections

Working Paper: The Effect of Electronic Voting Machines on Change in Support for Bush in the 2004 Florida Elections

FOREIGN FIRMS AND INDONESIAN MANUFACTURING WAGES: AN ANALYSIS WITH PANEL DATA

Main Tables and Additional Tables accompanying The Effect of FDI on Job Separation

TECHNICAL APPENDIX. Immigrant Earnings Growth: Selection Bias or Real Progress. Garnett Picot and Patrizio Piraino*

5.1 Assessing the Impact of Conflict on Fractionalization

Efficiency Consequences of Affirmative Action in Politics Evidence from India

THE FIELD POLL. By Mark DiCamillo, Director, The Field Poll

Ethnic Persistence, Assimilation and Risk Proclivity

Congruence in Political Parties

Understanding Subjective Well-Being across Countries: Economic, Cultural and Institutional Factors

A REPLICATION OF THE POLITICAL DETERMINANTS OF FEDERAL EXPENDITURE AT THE STATE LEVEL (PUBLIC CHOICE, 2005) Stratford Douglas* and W.

Practice Questions for Exam #2

PROJECTING THE LABOUR SUPPLY TO 2024

The impact of low-skilled labor migration boom on education investment in Nepal

Table A.2 reports the complete set of estimates of equation (1). We distinguish between personal

Learning from Small Subsamples without Cherry Picking: The Case of Non-Citizen Registration and Voting

Residential segregation and socioeconomic outcomes When did ghettos go bad?

44 th Congress of European Regional Science Association August 2004, Porto, Portugal

Allocating the US Federal Budget to the States: the Impact of the President. Statistical Appendix

FACTORS INFLUENCING VOTER TURNOUT IN LOCAL GOVERNMENT COUNCIL ELECTIONS IN UGANDA

NH Statewide Horserace Poll

MATS HAMMARSTEDT & CHIZHENG MIAO 2018:4. Self-employed immigrants and their employees Evidence from Swedish employer-employee data

Do Remittances Promote Household Savings? Evidence from Ethiopia

Corruption, Political Instability and Firm-Level Export Decisions. Kul Kapri 1 Rowan University. August 2018

Electoral competition and corruption: Theory and evidence from India

Brain drain and Human Capital Formation in Developing Countries. Are there Really Winners?

Determinants of Outward FDI for Thai Firms

Abstract for: Population Association of America 2005 Annual Meeting Philadelphia PA March 31 to April 2

Voting for Parties or for Candidates: Do Electoral Institutions Make a Difference?

Corruption and business procedures: an empirical investigation

Figure 2: Proportion of countries with an active civil war or civil conflict,

Michigan 14th Congressional District Democratic Primary Election Exclusive Polling Study for Fox 2 News Detroit.

How s Life in Norway?

Experiments in Election Reform: Voter Perceptions of Campaigns Under Preferential and Plurality Voting

Is there a Strategic Selection Bias in Roll Call Votes. in the European Parliament?

Non-Voted Ballots and Discrimination in Florida

Unequal Recovery, Labor Market Polarization, Race, and 2016 U.S. Presidential Election. Maoyong Fan and Anita Alves Pena 1

What about the Women? Female Headship, Poverty and Vulnerability

IS THERE A POLITICAL GENDER GAP IN UGANDA?

GOVERNANCE RETURNS TO EDUCATION: DO EXPECTED YEARS OF SCHOOLING PREDICT QUALITY OF GOVERNANCE?

1. Expand sample to include men who live in the US South (see footnote 16)

Rural and Urban Migrants in India:

Determinants and Effects of Negative Advertising in Politics

Supporting information

Europeans support a proportional allocation of asylum seekers

Supplementary Materials for

Interethnic Tolerance, Demographics, and the Electoral Fate of Non-nationalistic Parties in Post-war Bosnian Municipalities

Returns to Education in the Albanian Labor Market

A Vote Equation and the 2004 Election

The Effect of Ballot Order: Evidence from the Spanish Senate

Schooling and Cohort Size: Evidence from Vietnam, Thailand, Iran and Cambodia. Evangelos M. Falaris University of Delaware. and

How s Life in France?

262 Index. D demand shocks, 146n demographic variables, 103tn

CMIWORKINGPAPER. Explaining Maoist Control and Level of Civil Conflict in Nepal. Magnus Hatlebakk WP 2009: 10

The Wages of Religion

Education, Credentials and Immigrant Earnings*

Far Right Parties and the Educational Performance of Children *

Within-Groups Wage Inequality and Schooling: Further Evidence for Portugal

Transcription:

WP 2015: 9 Reported versus actual voting behaviour Ivar Kolstad and Arne Wiig VOTE

Chr. Michelsen Institute (CMI) is an independent, non-profit research institution and a major international centre in policy-oriented and applied development research. Focus is on development and human rights issues and on international conditions that affect such issues. The geographical focus is Sub-Saharan Africa, Southern and Central Asia, the Middle East and Latin America. CMI combines applied and theoretical research. CMI research intends to assist policy formulation, improve the basis for decision-making and promote public debate on international development issues.

Reported versus actual voting behaviour Ivar Kolstad** and Arne Wiig WP 2015:9 August 2015 * The authors thank Vincent Somville, Odd-Helge Fjeldstad and Lise Rakner for valuable comments. Financial support from the Research Council of Norway is gratefully acknowledged. ** Chr. Michelsen Institute, P.O. Box 6033, N-5892 Bergen, Norway. Phone: +47 47 93 81 22. Fax: +47 55 31 03 13. E-mail: ivar.kolstad@cmi.no *** Chr. Michelsen Institute, P.O. Box 6033, N-5892 Bergen, Norway. Phone: +47 47 93 81 22. Fax: +47 55 31 03 13. E-mail: arne.wiig@cmi.no.

Contents Abstract... 1 1. Introduction... 2 2. The data... 2 3. Results... 3 4. Concluding remarks... 5 References... 6 Appendix... 6 ii

CMI WP 2015:9 Reported versus actual voting behaviour www.cmi.no Abstract Using survey data of voters in Tanzania, this paper shows that while education does not affect selfreported voting in general elections, it increases actual voting. The less educated are more likely to claim to have voted without having done so, which may explain why previous studies of voting in developing countries fail to find an effect of education. We demonstrate the importance of this finding by using our survey data to generate predicted voting probabilities for the respondents to the 2012 Afrobarometer survey in Tanzania, and show that while mean self-reported voting does not differ much at different levels of education, the differences become significant when taking into account voting misrepresentation. Keywords: Voting, elections, participation, education, Tanzania JEL codes: C23, D72, F32, H11, H26 1

CMI WP 2015:9 Reported versus actual voting behaviour www.cmi.no 1. Introduction While it is typically found that more educated citizens are more likely to vote in elections in developed countries, results for developing countries are much more mixed and several recent studies suggest that education does not increase electoral participation (Isaksson, 2014; Isaksson et al, 2014; Croke et al., 2015). A problem in many empirical studies from developing countries is, however, a reliance on survey measures of self-reported voting, which are known to over-estimate voting (Aker et al., 2013). For instance, while more than 80 per cent of the respondents to the 2012 Afrobarometer survey in Tanzania claim to have voted in the 2010 general election, the actual turnout was about 40 per cent of the voting age population. If misrepresentation of voting is systematically related to education, with less educated citizens more likely to claim to have voted without having done so, this creates a downward bias in estimates of the relationship between education and voting. Using author collected survey data from Tanzania, we show that this is the case. We find education to be unrelated to self-reported voting, but be positively related to actual voting by our respondents, reflecting a greater prevalence of misreporting among those with less education. We show that our findings have substantial implications for studies using standard, nationally representative surveys to estimate the effect of education on voting, by generating predicted voting probabilities for the respondents to the 2012 Afrobarometer survey in Tanzania, demonstrating that these vary substantially across education levels, in contrast to self-reported voting. 2. The data In February 2015, we conducted a survey of a representative sample of the 146 voters in Dar Es Salaam, the major city in Tanzania. A random sample of 12 polling stations from the 2010 general election was drawn, from each of which our local teams of enumerators commenced walking, selecting first the 5 th household, and then every 10 th household on their way, interviewing one randomly selected adult respondent in each household. This approach is similar to that of the Afrobarometer surveys, and common in poor countries where electoral rolls are not accessible. We collected data on voting behaviour, educational attainment and a large set of background variables. Our main question on voting asked whether the respondent voted in the 2010 general election, but we also used a follow up multiple choice question where the respondent was asked what the ballot used in the elections looked like. For this follow up question, four options were given, of which only the second was correct (see the appendix for details). Our data hence allows us to distinguish respondents who have actually voted from those that merely claim to have voted. It is possible that those with low education have more trouble retaining information about the election and those with high education are better at educated guesswork. However, since we get actual participation rates close to the official turnout rates plus the expected proportion that manage to guess the correct answer to the follow up question, this would require a lot of successful guesswork on the part of the educated, which seems unlikely. The left half of Table 1 presents summary statistics for our survey. While 72 per cent of the respondents claimed to have voted in the 2010 election, almost a third of these answered the follow up question wrongly. The proportion of our respondents that we can say actually voted is therefore about 50 per cent, very close to actual turnout rates of about 40 per cent in the election, plus the expected rate of lucky guesses (if 40 per cent of our sample actually voted, a quarter of the remaining 32 per 2

CMI WP 2015:9 Reported versus actual voting behaviour www.cmi.no cent who claim to have voted can be expected to guess correctly on the follow up questions, i.e. about 8 per cent of the total sample). Table 1. Summary statistics Author survey sample Afrobarometer sample Variable Obs Mean Std. dev. Min Max Obs Mean Std. dev. Min Max Voted claimed 146 0.72 0.45 0 1 2146 0.81 0.39 0 1 Voted actual 146 0.50 0.50 0 1 Age 146 39.51 14.17 23 76 2146 38.75 14.52 18 99 Male 146 0.49 0.50 0 1 2146 0.49 0.50 0 1 Household size 146 2.93 1.61 1 9 2146 3.53 2.06 1 19 No education 146 0.08 0.26 0 1 2146 0.23 0.42 0 1 Primary education 146 0.58 0.50 0 1 2146 0.65 0.48 0 1 Secondary education 146 0.25 0.43 0 1 2146 0.12 0.32 0 1 Tertiary education 146 0.10 0.30 0 1 2146 0.01 0.10 0 1 Catholic 146 0.23 0.42 0 1 2146 0.29 0.45 0 1 Protestant 146 0.16 0.37 0 1 2146 0.26 0.44 0 1 Muslim 146 0.61 0.49 0 1 2146 0.45 0.50 0 1 We use dummies for whether respondents claim to have voted, whether they actually voted, and whether they misrepresented their voting as dependent variables in our empirical analysis. The other rows in Table 1 show the explanatory variables we include in our main specifications. These include the age of the respondent (and its square), gender, three education dummies for completed primary, secondary and tertiary education (the group with no education is the excluded category), and dummies for religious affiliation. Our main analysis includes variables that could also be generated from the 2012 Afrobarometer survey of Tanzania, since we want to use estimations from our survey data to predict probabilities that the respondents to the Afrobarometer actually voted, given that they claim to have voted. However, in additional estimations we have added further covariates, and results are essentially unchanged by adding marital status and headship status, income or asset ownership, occupation and polling station fixed effects (results available on request). The right half of Table 1 gives descriptive statistics for the Afrobarometer sample. A comparison of the two samples reveal that they are not too dissimilar, however, there are some differences reflecting the fact that our survey only included urban areas. We have therefore also performed our analysis of the Afrobarometer data using only respondents from Dar Es Salaam, and results are in essence the same (results available on request). 3. Results The main results using our survey data are presented in Table 2. All presented results are from linear probability models with robust standard errors; in additional estimations we have used probit estimation, with very similar results (results available on request). The first column of the table shows results from the estimation using a dummy for whether respondents claim to have voted. As we can see, none of the education variables have any significant association with whether respondents claim to have voted. The results from the second column, where the dependent variable is the dummy for whether respondents actually voted, are markedly different. Here all the education variables have significantly positive coefficients, indicating that respondents with primary, secondary and tertiary education are significantly more likely to actually vote than respondents with no completed education. Point estimates for secondary and tertiary education are also larger than for primary education, but the differences are not statistically significant. The third column highlights the reason behind the difference between the effect of education on claims of voting and on actual voting. The estimation here is on the subsample that claim to have voted, using a dummy for whether they misrepresented their voting behaviour as the dependent variable. The results show that those that have completed some level of education are far less likely to misrepresent their behaviour, to say they have voted 3

CMI WP 2015:9 Reported versus actual voting behaviour www.cmi.no when they have not. The difference between those with secondary and primary education is statistically significant in this case (p<.0702). Table 2. Main results from author survey sample, linear probability models Regression 1 Regression 2 Regression 3 Dependent variable Voted claimed Voted actual Misrepresentation Age 0.043*** 0.054*** -0.023 (0.01) (0.02) (0.02) Age squared -0.000** -0.001** 0.000 (0.00) (0.00) (0.00) Male -0.010 0.041-0.059 (0.09) (0.11) (0.10) Household size 0.021 0.017-0.004 (0.02) (0.02) (0.02) Primary education 0.126 0.385** -0.405** (0.12) (0.13) (0.17) Secondary education -0.003 0.457** -0.634*** (0.13) (0.16) (0.15) Tertiary education 0.061 0.438* -0.536*** (0.29) (0.23) (0.17) Catholic 0.061 0.109-0.061 (0.12) (0.09) (0.07) Protestant -0.028 0.022-0.046 (0.16) (0.14) (0.11) Constant -0.497-1.222** 1.286* (0.32) (0.46) (0.59) r2 0.182 0.129 0.138 N 146 146 105 Note: Standard errors clustered by polling station in parentheses, *** indicates significance at the 1% level, ** at 5%, * at 10%. Our results hence show a systematic pattern in misreporting of past voting behaviour, which influences estimates of the effect of education on voting. An implication of this is that using standard, nationally representative surveys of voting behaviour may be associated with substantial bias. To demonstrate that this is the case, we use regressions on our smaller survey sample to generate predicted voting probabilities for the respondents to the 2012 Afrobarometer survey for Tanzania, given their background characteristics. In this case, we ran a probit regression with the above specification to keep predicted values between 0 and 1, and used the resulting estimates to produce predicted voting probabilities for the Afrobarometer respondents. Since it would not be all that meaningful to use these predicted probabilities in regressions, we have calculated the unconditional means of the voting probabilities across the four different education levels. The results are shown in Figure 1. 4

CMI WP 2015:9 Reported versus actual voting behaviour www.cmi.no Figure 1. Claimed and predicted voting by education group using Afrobarometer sample Voting claims Voting predictions Mean 0.2.4.6.8 1 Mean 0.2.4.6.8 1 None Primary Secondary Tertiary Level of education 95% confidence intervals None Primary Secondary Tertiary Level of education 95% confidence intervals The left hand panel of Figure 1 shows the means of claimed voting in the Afrobarometer sample, and the 95 per cent confidence interval, across the four levels of education. The means for all groups are between 0.72 and 0.83, and higher for the groups with no or primary education than for those with secondary or tertiary. The means for the primary education group is even significantly higher than for the secondary education one (p<.0003, one-sided t-test), suggesting decreasing voting towards the higher end of education. However, the predicted voting probabilities shown in the right hand panel of Figure 1 paints a quite different picture. This essentially shows means corrected for misrepresentation of voting in the four education groups. Mean predicted voting for those with secondary education is significantly higher than for those with none or primary education (p<.0000 and p<.0022, respectively, in one-sided t-tests). Correcting for voting misrepresentation, there is therefore substantial shift in levels of voting by education group, suggesting an increasing association with education rather than a decreasing one. We note that mean predicted voting across all respondents in the Afrobarometer survey is 49 per cent, much closer to the actual turnout in the 2010 election than the mean voting claim of 81 per cent. 4. Concluding remarks This paper has shown that misreporting of voting is systematically related to respondents education levels, creating a downward bias in estimates of effects of education on electoral participation. While our results show that the less educated are more likely to say they have voted when they have not, our data does not really tell us much about the reasons for this. One possibility could be social desirability bias increasing in the relative social distance to the enumerators conducting the interviews. Our results suggest that education may be more important for political participation in a developing country context than previous studies suggest, and is consistent with recent findings of effects of voter information and education campaigns (Aker et al., 2013). While we have focused on education, the unreliability of self-reported voting as a proxy for actual voting in standard surveys, could also affect estimates of results of other potential determinants of voting. 5

CMI WP 2015:9 Reported versus actual voting behaviour www.cmi.no References Aker, J. C., Collier, P. and Vicente, P. C., (2013) Is Information Power? Using Mobile Phones and Free Newspapers During an Election in Mozambique (May 1, 2013). NOVAFRICA Working Paper Series No. 1304. Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=2384596 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2384596 Croke, K., Grossman, G., Larreguy, H. A. and Marshall, J. (2015), Deliberate disengagement: How education decreases political participation in electoral authoritarian regimes, Working Paper No. 156, www.afrobarometer.org Isaksson, A. S. (2014), Political participation in Africa: The role of individual resources, Electoral Studies, 34, 244-260 Isaksson, A. S., Kotsadam, A. and Nerman, M. (2014), The gender gap in African political participation: Testing theories of individual and contextual determinants, Journal of Development Studies, 50, 2, 302-318 Appendix Follow up question on voting (English translation) What did the ballots look like? 1. Only the name of the candidate was on the ballot. 2. The name of the candidate with the photo and the name of the party were on the ballot. 3. The name of the candidate with the photo were on the ballot. 4. The name of the candidate and the name of the party were on the ballot. 6

CMI WORKING PAPERS This series can be ordered from: CMI (Chr. Michelsen Institute) Phone: +47 47 93 80 00 Fax: +47 47 93 80 01 E-mail: cmi@cmi.no P.O.Box 6033 Bedriftssenteret, N-5892 Bergen, Norway Visiting address: Jekteviksbakken 31, Bergen Web: www.cmi.no Printed version: ISSN 0804-3639 Electronic version: ISSN 1890-5048 Printed version: ISBN 978-82-8062-554-0 Electronic version: ISBN 978-82-8062-555-7 This paper is also available at: www.cmi.no/publications INDEXING TERMS Voting, elections, participation, education, Tanzania

Using survey data of voters in Tanzania, this paper shows that while education does not affect self-reported voting in general elections, it increases actual voting. The less educated are more likely to claim to have voted without having done so, which may explain why previous studies of voting in developing countries fail to find an effect of education. We demonstrate the importance of this finding by using our survey data to generate predicted voting probabilities for the respondents to the 2012 Afrobarometer survey in Tanzania, and show that while mean self-reported voting does not differ much at different levels of education, the differences become significant when taking into account voting misrepresentation.