By: Stan V. Smith, Ph.D.

Similar documents
"Measuring The Loss of Enjoyment of Life in Personal Injury Cases in Washington - Hedonic Damages, "

MODEL MOTOR VEHICLE NEGLIGENCE CHARGE AND VERDICT SHEET. MOTOR VEHICLE VOLUME REPLACEMENT JUNE

MODEL JURY SELECTION QUESTIONS

PRETRIAL INSTRUCTIONS. CACI No. 100

GENERAL CLOSING INSTRUCTIONS. Members of the jury, it is now time for me to tell you the law that applies to

Federal Tort Trials and Verdicts,

Second, you must not be influenced by sympathy, passion or prejudice in favor of any party or against any of the parties.

IN THE CIRCUIT COURT OF CLAY COUNTY, LIBERTY, MISSOURI. Case No. Division

2:16-cv EIL # 106 Page 1 of 20

JURY SELECTION: YOUR LAST LINE OF DEFENSE

The HIDDEN COST Of Proving Your Innocence

This memo was published originally as Appendix C to the 1996 Report of the Governor s Advisory Task Force on Civil Justice Reform.

JUDGE DENISE POSSE LINDBERG STOCK CIVIL JURY INSTRUCTIONS TABLE OF CONTENTS

Function of the Jury Burden of Proof and Greater Weight of the Evidence Credibility of Witness Weight of the Evidence

WILLIAM MICHAEL BOYKIN, Plaintiff, v. THOMAS RAY MORRISON, RUFUS AARON WILSON, JR. and WILLIE PERRY, Defendants No. COA (Filed 28 December 2001)

Fall 1995 December 15, 1995 SAMPLE ANSWER TO MID-TERM EXAM QUESTION 1

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE WESTERN DISTRICT OF PENNSYLVANIA

The Below Average Defendant: Establishing BAC Evidence in DUI Cases

Chapter 3 The Court System and Chapter 4 The Litigation Process

Punitive damages in insurance bad-faith cases after State Farm v. Campbell

Case 3:12-cv Document 1 Filed 11/15/12 Page 1 of 17

JURY SELECTION (CRIMINAL)

FALL 2001 December 15, 2001 FALL SEMESTER SAMPLE ANSWER

Index. Belief in a just world, 149 Bench trial, , 257 agreement with jury decisions, Benevolent gestures, , 168

v. Record No OPINION BY JUSTICE ELIZABETH B. LACY April 23, 2004 ALBERT R. MARSHALL

The Culture of Modern Tort Law

Record No Circuit Court No. CL12-122

Robert I, Duke of Normandy. 22 June July 1035

IN THE COURT OF APPEALS OF TENNESSEE AT KNOXVILLE September 16, 2013 Session

Why Good Judges and Lawyers Make Bad Decisions

NO. 07-CI JEFFERSON CIRCUIT COURT DIVISION TEN (10) JUDGE IRV MAZE TONIA FREEMAN PLAINTIFF. BECKER LAW OFFICE, PLC, et al.

MODEL JURY SELECTION QUESTIONS FOR CIVIL TRIALS

Complaint - Walmart Substance on Floor in Frozen Food Dept.

7.32 COMPARATIVE NEGLIGENCE: INTERROGATORIES (Approved before 1985) NOTE TO JUDGE

ERIKA DuBOIS, as Guardian Ad Litem of KORIN DuBOIS, a Minor, Appellant, v. RICHARD GRANT, Respondent. No July 21, P.

Damages Pt. 2 Duty to Mitigate Damages

THE IMPACT OF TAXES ON MIGRATION IN NEW HAMPSHIRE

13 ADVANCED TRIAL TIPS. Gary K. Burger BURGER LAW BurgerLaw.com

Legal Change: Integrating Selective Litigation, Judicial Preferences, and Precedent

HINTS FROM A NEUTRAL MEDIATOR (CASE EVALUATOR) BY: MARTIN I. REISIG

THE WORKMEN S CIRCLE SURVEY OF AMERICAN JEWS. Jews, Economic Justice & the Vote in Steven M. Cohen and Samuel Abrams

Case 1:17-cv WYD-SKC Document 150 Filed 02/19/19 USDC Colorado Page 1 of 32 JURY INSTRUCTIONS

IN THE CIRCUIT COURT OF JACKSON COUNTY, MISSOURI AT KANSAS CITY PETITION

FOR IMMEDIATE NEWS RELEASE NEWS RELEASE # 80 FROM: CLERK OF SUPREME COURT OF LOUISIANA

JURY SELECTION AFTER CORTEZ

SUMMER 1995 August 11, 1995 SAMPLE ANSWER TO FINAL EXAM

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE DISTRICT OF COLORADO

Printable Lesson Materials

Texas Law & Due Process (Chapter 10) Dr. Michael Sullivan. Texas State Government GOVT

IN THE SUPREME COURT OF TEXAS

THE STATE OF NEW HAMPSHIRE. Lincoln & Carol Hanscom. Linda O Connell. No. 03-C-338 ORDER

CASE SCENARIO #1. Did the court commit an error in refusing to set aside the default? Even if not, would you have acted differently?

EXAM NO. UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, HASTINGS COLLEGE OF THE LAW FINAL EXAMINATION

For Preview Only - Please Do Not Copy

THE JURY EFFECT ON PUNITIVE DAMAGES: AN EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS. Kenneth M. Grose *

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT EASTERN DISTRICT OF WISCONSIN. COMPLAINT Plaintiffs, v.

CONDENSED OUTLINE FOR TORTS I

Population Vitality Overview

STATE OF NORTH CAROLINA TRANSPORTATION COMPENDIUM OF LAW

PRESS RELEASE October 15, 2008

Damage Strategies Beyond Back and Front Pay Wage Recoveries and the Role of Expert Witnesses: the Plaintiff s Perspective

Wert v. Mesesick, No CnC (Katz, J., Apr. 7, 2005)

Truck Accident Litigation in the SML Footprint:

IN THE CIRCUIT COURT OF COOK COUNTY, ILLINOIS COUNTY DEPARTMENT, LAW DIVISION

NANCY MAE GILLIAM OPINION BY v. Record No JUSTICE ELIZABETH A. McCLANAHAN January 19, 2017 JACOB THOMAS IMMEL

Lewis v New York City Tr. Auth NY Slip Op 33280(U) December 19, 2013 Supreme Court, New York County Docket Number: /06 Judge: Paul Wooten

Essentials of Demonstrative Evidence

Adding Vulnerable Victim to the Physical Injury Statute ORS

RESEARCH BRIEF: The State of Black Workers before the Great Recession By Sylvia Allegretto and Steven Pitts 1

As Introduced. Regular Session H. B. No

Interrogatories. As I have previously written, interrogatories are one. The building blocks of your client s case. Discovery. by Thomas J.

Albanian-American Diaspora Survey Report

Institute for Public Policy and Economic Analysis

Negotiation, Settlement and the Contingent Fee

NEGLIGENCE. All four of the following must be demonstrated for a legal claim of negligence to be successful:

TORT LAW. By Helen Jordan, Elaine Martinez, and Jim Ponce

LEGAL GLOSSARY Additur Adjudication Admissible evidence Advisement Affiant - Affidavit - Affirmative defense - Answers to Interrogatories - Appeal -

Community Well-Being and the Great Recession

SUPREME COURT OF MISSOURI en banc

California Bar Examination

Medical Malpractice in Israel and the Financial and Non-financial Damage to the Victim

Overview of the Jury System. from the Perspective of a Korean Attorney. From the perspective of a Korean attorney, the jury system

2016 Nova Scotia Culture Index

COPYRIGHT 2009 THE LAW PROFESSOR

Coming to a person s aid when off duty

9 of their attorneys you have learned the conclusion which 10 each party believes should be drawn from the evidence

Pennsylvania Bar Association 100 South Street P.O. Box 186 Harrisburg, PA (800)

STRENGTHENING RURAL CANADA: Fewer & Older: The Coming Population and Demographic Challenges in Rural Newfoundland & Labrador

a. To effect an arrest or bring a subject under control;

The Civil Action Part 1 of a 4 part series

Civil procedure Professor Perritt Fall 2017 Model Answer

Fall 1994 December 12, 1994 SAMPLE ANSWER TO MID-TERM EXAM QUESTION 1

Litigation Unveiled Click to edit Master title style

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT NORTHERN DISTRICT OF CALIFORNIA

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE DISTRICT OF COLUMBIA CIVIL DIVISION

LAW DIVISION: MORRIS COUNTY DOCKET NO.: MRS-L CIVIL ACTION. Plaintiff, Richard Balestrino, residing in Vernon, Sussex

Chandanais v Ryan 2010 NY Slip Op 32202(U) August 18, 2010 Supreme Court, Broome County Docket Number: Judge: Ferris D. Lebous Republished

CLOSING INSTRUCTIONS. this case. As I mentioned at the beginning of the trial, you must keep an open

IN THE COURT OF APPEALS OF TENNESSEE AT NASHVILLE August 4, 2009 Session

The Eyewitness Dilemma: Offering Evidence of Automobile Speed Through an Expert Witness

Transcription:

"Why Juries Can Be Trusted," Voir Dire, Vol. 5, Issue 3, Summer 1998, pp. 19-21 & 25, American Board of Trial Advocates. By: Stan V. Smith, Ph.D. Introduction Setting aside the one case in 100,000 that makes headlines, are juries generally capricious and liberal? Are verdicts frequently unreasonable? In order to effectively assess the quality of the civil jury system, there is a compelling need for research on the quality of jury decision-making, rather than anecdotal, if spectacular, cases. Whatever the solution, contemplating altering the rules of the civil jury system ought to be based on a careful examination of the outcomes generated by the jury decision-making process, rather than on anecdotal, emotionally manipulative stories of supposed jury incompetence. My own research, in fact, provides strong evidence that juries are rational, restrained, and thoughtful, that their verdicts are quite predictable. The awards they determine can be mostly anticipated and are based on factors that juries should be considering to determine awards. If anything, juries err on the side of the defendant, not the plaintiff. Last year, I completed a doctorate in economics at the University of Chicago, for which I wrote a thesis based on an empirical examination of the civil jury system. I was fortunate to have my research supervised by several of the world's premier economists, including the 1992 Nobel Laureate in Economics.1 In my dissertation, I analyzed compensatory and punitive damages awarded by juries for claims arising from injuries sustained in automobile accident cases involving allegations of driving under the influence of alcohol. My objective was to determine if juries were making rational, well-thought-out decisions in awarding compensatory damages. In this article, I give a synopsis of the results of my research and the relevance of these findings to tort reform efforts. Previous Research on Jury Decision-making Ideally, to assess how juries make decisions, we would like to observe their actual behavior in the jury room. Unfortunately, the jury deliberation process is hidden from view. As a matter of fact, for a short time in the mid-1950s, researchers at the University of Chicago were permitted to listen in on the deliberations of several federal juries, but a subsequent Congressional inquiry soon led to legislation prohibiting this. Thus, exactly how juries reach decisions inside the jury room cannot be observed or determined directly, which presents a thorny research problem: We can observe what is presented

to a jury, and we can observe the results of the decisions that juries make, but actual deliberations are hidden to researchers. We can only infer what factors they base their decisions upon. But, by using econometric analysis, powerful conclusions can be reached based on standard statistical research methodologies. Among the prior research papers published in this area, there are two notable studies that were highly informative and report findings consistent with that of other jury verdict research. An economist at Harvard University, Kip Viscusi, examined over 11,000 insurance claims and evaluated all claims that paid for pain and suffering.2 The results of his research led him to reject the idea that payments for pain and suffering awards are arbitrary and capricious. In fact, the average payment was rather modest, amounting to approximately $18,000 in 1986 dollars. Viscusi argues that proposed limits or caps on awards for pain and suffering would negatively impact the few victims of catastrophic injuries, such as brain injury and quadriplegia, while leaving the great majority of awards for lesser injuries unaffected. He found that larger claims for pain and suffering do not receive proportionally larger awards. A researcher at the RAND Corporation, Robert MacCoun, reviewed a large body of jury verdicts and observed, among other things, that fewer than 9 percent of cases involve punitive awards, and the median in 15 of 20 jurisdictions was below $40,000.3 He also found that over 50 percent of punitive awards were reduced or eliminated in post-trial proceedings, that wealthy defendants do not have a different outcome than other noninstitutional defendants, and that while juries tend to award more money than judges, judges tend to find for the plaintiffs more often. MacCoun concluded that there was no evidence to suggest that jurors are less competent than judges as fact finders and cites evidence for advantages in the performance of heterogeneous groups over that of individuals. What do juries count as important? How much of their decisions can be explained by observable factors? These are important questions to be addressed in assessing the quality of the civil jury system. They are questions I sought to answer in my own research. A Model of the Jury Decision In a typical (non-fatal) personal injury case, plaintiffs make claims in several categories for compensation resulting from losses: first, past and future lost earnings; second, household service losses; third, medical and property losses; and fourth, pain and suffering, including loss of enjoyment of life. These four categories and the factors that affect them are generally observable in every trial and are available, to varying degrees, in the Jury Verdict Research (JVR) data. Claims for compensation can include any one or more of these categories. The job of the jury is to determine the extent of losses in each category, based on the evidence provided by the plaintiff and the defense. In my statistical analysis I take into consideration these factors through the use of multiple regression, a statistical method which allows me to measure the separate effect of many possible factors that impact the jury's decision. In the process of determining these effects, I can also estimate what portion of

the award that cannot be explained by the factors-a way of measuring how "rational" juries are. The greater the unexplainable portion, the more irrational are juries and visa versa. In the next section, I give a brief overview of the results of my research. The Data To analyze how juries determine awards for damages, I examined data obtained from Jury Verdict Research, Inc. CJVR) on 666 nonfatal, drunkdriving cases occurring in the time period from 1980 through 1990. JVR collects data on a nationwide basis from all 50 states and estimates that it gathers information on approximately 40 percent of all verdicts.4 In addition, to be able to capture socioeconomic factors not available from the JVR data (probable racial composition of the jury. average income, etc.), 1 also used U.S. Census data for 1980 and 1990. For each case, JVR reports the total award and its components (wage, medical, etc.); the outcome reached (verdict, settlement, etc.); nature of the liability (contributory negligence, punitive damages, mental impairment of the plaintiff and offender, etc.); the age, gender and occupation of the plaintiff; whether the defendant was an individual or institution; a description of the injury; and other data related to the case. Reported award values from the JVR data ranged from under $1,000 to over $23 million, with a mean of just over $1 million. In order to be able to compare the cases to each other more easily, I used a single, whole-body measurement of impairment, developed for the US Department of Transportation and other Federal Agencies, that incorporates all of the injuries received by the victim and generates an index measurement going from 0 (no impairment) to 1 (complete impairment).5 Research Results The single most important result of the study is that it is possible to predict with a high degree of accuracy what the jury's award decision will be, based on the observed, objective factors reported in JVR data. Over 75 percent of the variation amongst awards can be explained by these observed factors. In other words, to a surprisingly large degree, juries base their decisions on the actual facts of the case. There appears to be little subjectivity or emotion involved in the decision for damages. Another key finding is that juries base most of the award on the nature and type of injury. This is highly reassuring since it is the injury itself that gives rise to the lawsuit and serves as the basis for compensatory claims. It is the jury's job to compensate for loss based on the factual evidence presented regarding the injury. This finding was very consistent in my results. A further key finding is that juries give greater weight to aspects of the injury that they can directly see. That is, victims whose impairments affected their mobility (walking, etc.), senses (e.g., loss of sight), and / or their appearance (visible scarring) received larger awards than victims whose impairments were just as severe on the impairment scale but not so easy to observe, such as cognition (e.g., memory loss, inability to concentrate), pain (chronic neck pain resulting from the accident), and work-related impairments that affect the victim's ability to return to work. This suggests a conservative,

skeptical and suspicious approach to injury evaluation. I also checked to see if the magnitude of the award was consistent with the loss of enjoyment of life or pain and suffering associated with injury. This is an area of jury decision making most frequently criticized by tort reformers. As noted above, included in my data was a comprehensive measure of impairment arising from the injury, with a range from 0 to 1. A rating of 1 essentially equivalent to death. Using this measure, I calculated the jury's implied value of life. As an example, if a plaintiff has an impairment rated at 0.5 (50% loss) and the jury awards $2 million, then the jury's implied value of life is $4 million, I found that juries calculated awards that implied a value of human life between $2.2 to $4.8 million. This valuation of life is totally consistent with the statistical value of life estimated by academic researchers and used as standards by US Government Agencies and US industry.6,7 Juries take income losses into account in very sophisticated ways, as they should. People in lower-income jobs received smaller awards, as one would expect. Juries are also conscious of the age of the plaintiffs and made the largest awards to plaintiffs who are around 40 years old. This is consistent with accepted economic theory and research: significantly younger plaintiffs are awarded less in general; their careers are less certain and they are likely to have fewer dependents. Significantly older plaintiffs are also awarded less; the present value of their remaining work life is less and they also have fewer dependents. I also found that juries do not decrease the compensatory component of the award prior to their assessment of contributory negligence by the plaintiff. They seem to be following the judge's instructions to determine the award based on the victim's loss, and then determine if the victim's behavior helped lead to the accident. Further, juries react neutrally to allegations that the defendant was impaired at the time of the accident, even if the defendant is later convicted of driving under the influence. These two findings show a rather incredible ability to mentally separate liability issues from damages measurement. Juries do appear to reduce compensation for plaintiffs who have been alleged to be impaired at the time of incident. Apparently the behavior of the defendants affects issues of liability only, while possible impairment of the plaintiff does affect the award. These results lend further support for the conclusion that juries are conservative in making awards. Juries appear to compensate for medical costs on a one-for-one basis, suggesting a fairly rational, cost accounting, approach to the verdict. They do, however, increase the compensatory component when punitive damages are assessed. The results also show that when juries are allocating damages, they are not overloading in one area of the award (say, punitive damages) in order to avoid a cap or other limit on damages in another area (compensation). Finally, juries award more conservatively against individual defendants, perhaps based a perceived lower ability to pay. Since assessing compensation involves estimation, errors on the low side are apparently made regarding awards against individuals. Conclusion The overall pattern of results fits that of a system where, in general,

rationality and thoughtful process are the standards in many key aspects of decision making. There is nothing in the results that constitutes an urgent call for meddling in the civil jury system. To the contrary, there is much evidence for trusting juries to be fair and even restrained. Most of the verdict is predictable based on the extent of injury, medical costs and lost income, indicating rational decision making. There are several conservative tendencies: Non-observable aspects of the injury are given much less weight. The award is lower if the plaintiff was alleged to be intoxicated or if the defendant is an individual. Further, juries appear to be scrupulous in not allowing any impact on the award to arise from liability issues involving the defendant's intoxication, defendant's conviction for drunken driving, or plaintiff's contributory negligence. Only when egregious defendant behavior warrants punitive damages do compensatory damages tend to become more liberal. What emerges from this research is a picture of a jury as a deliberative, restrained body, which takes into account relevant information and bases compensation primarily on measures relating to the degree of injury to the body and the ability to earn. What this study has found is overwhelmingly reassuring about jury behavior. Juries appear to be neither capricious nor liberal, but thoughtful and conservative in general. Americans are activists. Our democracy was born in rebellion. Sometimes we rush to reform. From Sinclair Lewis' portrayal of the meat industry in the 1880s to current complaints of excess election campaign spending, our institutions are constantly being examined and frequently condemned. Sometimes the need for reform is legitimate. However, reform should neither precede thoughtful analysis of the problem nor ignore evidence of the system working. Sound evidence and thoughtful analysis are required to ensure that appropriate actions are taken. The jury system is grounded in a profound trust in the common sense of our fellow citizens. My research shows that placing faith in our fellow citizens may make much more sense than trusting professional politicians to second guess juries. The system is not only not broken, it is revealed to be working remarkably well. Economist Stan V. Smith is president of Corporate Financial Group in Chicago, a nationally recognized economic consulting firm offering consulting services and Litigation support in economics and finance to defense as well as plaintiff counsel nationwide. Dr. Smith received his Ph.D. in Economics from the University of Chicago and has written many articles and co-authored a textbook on Economic/Hedonic Damages. He and his work have been profiled in the Wall Street Journal, the American Bar Association Journal, the National Law Journal, Trial, the publication of the Association of Trial Lawyers of America, and in many law review articles. Endnotes (From Juries Can Be Trusted) 1. Professor Gary Becker was awarded the Nobel Prize in Economics in 1992. My four dissertation committee members at the University of Chicago were responsible for assuring that I followed sound research practices. The opinions expressed in this paper are my own.

2. Viscusi, Kip W., "Pain and Suffering in Product Liability Cases: Systematic Compensation or Capricious Awards?" International Review of Law and Economics. 8, 1988. pp. 203-220 3. MacCoun, Robert, "Inside the Black Box. What Empirical Research Tells Us About Decision making by Civil Juries," in Ulan, Robert E., ed., Verdict: Assessing the Civil fury System, Washington. D.C: The Brookings Institution, 1993. 4. Cohen, Mark A. and Ted R. Miller. "The Monetary Value of Pain and Suffering due to Criminal Victimization: Evidence from Jury Awards," Working Paper, 1994. 5. Miller, Ted R. et al., "The Cost of Highway Crashes," final Report to Federal Highway Administration, The Urban Institute, 1991, and Rossman Shelli B., Ted R. Miller, and John B. Douglass, The Cost of Occupational Musculoskeletal and Severe Occupational Traumatic Injuries. Final Report to NIOSH. Washinglon.D.C.: The Urban Institute, 1991. 6. Miller, Ted R-, "The Plausible Range for the Value of Life; Red Herrings Among the Mackerel, "journal of forensic Economics. 3(3), 1990, pp. 17-39. 7. Gillette, Clayton P. and Thomas D Hopkins, "Federal Agency Valuations of Human Life," Report to the Administrative Conference of the United Slates. July 7,1988.