Living with others: Mapping the routes to acculturation in a multicultural society

Similar documents
Attitudes towards influx of immigrants in Korea

Refugee and Migrant Integration: Examining the discourse of the dominant

Being Prepared For Acculturation: On the Importance of the First Months After Immigrants. Enter a New Culture. Marcella Ramelli

Intercultural Relations in a Prairie City

Acculturation over time among adolescents from immigrant Chinese families

ACCULTURATION AND INTERCULTURAL PERCEPTIONS: What I think, what you think, what I think you think and why it s all important

Hosting asylum seekers and attitudes toward cultural diversity in. Australia

Acculturation, Identity and Wellbeing among Ethnocultural Youth

Congregational Attitudes toward Immigrants: The Case of Australian Churches Century?

Acculturation attitude and its effect on work engagement: a case of professional Chinese immigrants in Australia

Basic Elements of an Immigration Analysis

Acculturation Orientations towards Valued and Devalued Immigrants in South Korea

The role of immigrant acculturation preferences and generational status in determining majority intergroup attitudes

The impact of multiculturalism on immigrant helping

Assessment of Immigration and Acculturation

COMMUNITY PERCEPTIONS OF MIGRANTS AND IMMIGRATION

Intercultural relations in Russia and Latvia: the relationship between contact and cultural security

ATTITUDINAL DIVERGENCE IN A MELBOURNE REGION OF HIGH IMMIGRANT CONCENTRATION: A CASE STUDY

Acculturation Strategies : The Case of the Muslim Minority in the United States

University of Groningen. Attachment in cultural context Polek, Elzbieta

8th International Metropolis Conference, Vienna, September 2003

Does Framing Integration in Pro-Diversity Terms Improve Attitudes Toward. Interculturalism. Colin Scott

Problems Immigrants Face In Host Countries Jabr Almutairi, Kingston University Of London, United Kingdom

ASSESSING THE INTENDED PARTICIPATION OF YOUNG ADOLESCENTS AS FUTURE CITIZENS: COMPARING RESULTS FROM FIVE EAST ASIAN COUNTRIES

Acculturation on Stress, Quality of Life, and Self-Esteem in Married Immigrant Women in Korea

Cultural Identity of Migrants in USA and Canada

Legal Studies. Stage 6 Syllabus

Media Influence on Host Society Responsibility in the Integration of Immigrants

Bachelor of Social Sciences (Honours)

Journal of Cross-Cultural Family Studies. Jennifer L. Wilson South Texas Veteran s Health Care System,

The nature of acculturation and its relationship to health

De-coding Australian opinion: Australians and cultural diversity. Professor Andrew Markus

Investigating the dynamics of migration and health in Australia: A Longitudinal study

Rethinking Australian Migration

Anti-immigration populism: Can local intercultural policies close the space? Discussion paper

THE NORTHERN TERRITORY S RY S OVERSEAS BORN POPULATION

Real Adaption or Not: New Generation Internal Migrant Workers Social Adaption in China

IMMIGRANT STUDENTS ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE IN AUSTRALIA, NEW ZEALAND, CANADA AND SINGAPORE

Statements of Learning for Civics and Citizenship

Immigration and Multiculturalism: Views from a Multicultural Prairie City

Antonio J. Rojas a, Marisol Navas a, Pablo Sayans-Jiménez a & Isabel Cuadrado a a University of Almería

Community perceptions of migrants and immigration. D e c e m b e r

Team sport and acculturation in multicultural societies: The perspective of the dominant culture

International Dialogue on Migration Intersessional workshop on Societies and identities: the multifaceted impact of migration

The Adaptive Value of Ethnic, National and Multicultural Orientations for Immigrants and Nationals in the U.S.

2014 Migration Update Report

Political Integration of Immigrants: Insights from Comparing to Stayers, Not Only to Natives. David Bartram

Democratic Support among Youth in Some East Asian Countries

UTS:IPPG Project Team. Project Director: Associate Professor Roberta Ryan, Director IPPG. Project Manager: Catherine Hastings, Research Officer

POLICY BRIEF. Australian Population & Migration Research Centre. By Justin Civitillo

Deakin Research Online

Transnational Ties of Latino and Asian Americans by Immigrant Generation. Emi Tamaki University of Washington

Migrant s insertion and settlement in the host societies as a multifaceted phenomenon:

Submission to the Productivity Commission inquiry into Australia s Migrant Intake

Host Acculturation Orientation: Some Preliminary Impressions of the French Students on Ethnic Minority Groups in Montpellier, S.

NATIONAL TRAVELLER WOMENS FORUM

Main findings of the joint EC/OECD seminar on Naturalisation and the Socio-economic Integration of Immigrants and their Children

Team Australia? : Understanding Acculturation From Multiple Perspectives

Madrid Statement on ASEM Interfaith Dialogue

Professor Andre M.N. Renzaho, PhD

ANNE MONSOUR, Not Quite White: Lebanese and the White Australia Policy, 1880 to 1947 (Brisbane: Post Pressed, 2010). Pp $45.65 paper.

Refugees and regional settlement: win win?

ACCULTURATION JOURNEYS OF INTERNATIONAL FACULTY: RESULTS FROM A NARRATIVE QUALITATIVE STUDY AT TWO MIDWEST RESEARCH UNIVERSITIES

Unity Out of Diversity first results 26 October 2015

The Development of Australian Internal Migration Database

Transforming Halifax. Connecting Immigrants with Planning Policy and Urban Design. Cities & Environment Unit

Obstacles Facing Jordanian Women s Participation in the Political Life from the Perspective of Female Academic Staff in the Jordanian Universities

A National Action Plan to Build on Social Cohesion, Harmony and Security

Public Opinion & Political Development in Hong Kong. Survey Results. September 21, 2014

Economic correlates of Net Interstate Migration to the NT (NT NIM): an exploratory analysis

Acculturation and Social Cohesion: Emerging Issues for Asian Immigrants in New Zealand 1

Acculturation Strategies in relation to Economic and Psychological Adaptation of Immigrants in Greece

Immigration, Community and Ethnic Diversity

THE IPSOS MACKAY REPORT SBS IMMIGRATION NATION

ATTITUDES TOWARDS IMMIGRATION: ECONOMIC VERSUS CULTURAL DETERMINANTS. EVIDENCE FROM THE 2011 TRANSATLANTIC TRENDS IMMIGRATION DATA

BIRTHPLACE ORIGINS OF AUSTRALIA S IMMIGRANTS

Georg Lutz, Nicolas Pekari, Marina Shkapina. CSES Module 5 pre-test report, Switzerland

RAIS RESEARCH. An Influx of Refugees into Jeju Island and its Effects on the Jeju Residents Cultural Sensitivity. Yun Bin Cho 1, Yun Gi Cho 2

ETHNO-CULTURAL IDENTITY CONFLICT IN THE ACCULTURATION PROCESS

TAIWAN. CSES Module 5 Pretest Report: August 31, Table of Contents

POLITICAL SCIENCE (POLI)

The Economic and Social Outcomes of Children of Migrants in New Zealand

Languages of work and earnings of immigrants in Canada outside. Quebec. By Jin Wang ( )

In 2000, an estimated 175 million people lived outside their place of birth, more than

SOCIO-EDUCATIONAL SUPPORT OPPORTUNITIES FOR YOUNG JOB EMIGRANTS IN THE CONTEXT OF ANOTHER CULTURAL ENVIRONMENT

Challenges of International Career for Teachers

Supporting People from Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Backgrounds (CLDB) to be Part of Australian Society

Islamic and Chinese minorities as an integration paradox?

IFSAM 2002 Conference Gold Coast, Queensland Australia

IS CHINA S SOFT POWER DOMINATING SOUTHEAST ASIA? VIEWS FROM THE CITIZENS

Paper presented by Dr James Jupp (Australian National University) The overall policies of the Commonwealth government under the immigration power

Programme Specification

Public Attitudes to Migrant Workers. Please do not quote or publish without prior permission from the ILO

Investigation of Consumer Acculturation in Dining-out: a Comparison. between Recent Chinese Immigrants and Established Chinese Immigrants in

Sample. The Political Role of Freedom and Equality as Human Values. Marc Stewart Wilson & Christopher G. Sibley 1

A Study on the Relationship between Chinese Proficiency and the Leisure Constraints of Vietnamese Female Immigrants in Taiwan

Social Science Survey Data Sets in the Public Domain: Access, Quality, and Importance. David Howell The Philippines September 2014

Chinese Ethnic Economy in Toronto

Visibility, loss of status and life satisfaction in three groups of recent refugee settlers

Counseling Competence Scale on Refugees The researchers report the development of the Counseling Competence Scale on Refugees (CCSR) to respond to

Transcription:

ARTICLE IN PRESS International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 www.elsevier.com/locate/ijintrel Living with others: Mapping the routes to acculturation in a multicultural society Shuang Liu School of Journalism and Communication, The University of Queensland, Brisbane QLD 4072, Australia Accepted 29 August 2007 Abstract This study investigated attitudes towards multiculturalism and their influence on acculturation strategies of both Anglo-Australians and Asian immigrants residing in the city of Brisbane, the third largest city of Australia. Data was obtained via a survey administered to 133 Asian immigrants and 108 Anglo-Australians, a total of 241 respondents. Results revealed discordance in attitudes towards multiculturalism between Asians and Australians, with Asians rating it higher as a benefit and lower as a threat as compared to Australians. While higher ratings on multiculturalism as a threat tended to be positively related to separation strategy, this linear association did not hold true for the minority group (Asians). For Asian respondents, those who perceived a moderate threat in multiculturalism were more likely supporters for separation. Our findings supported the assumption that multiculturalism is viewed as differentially beneficial for minority and majority groups. r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Acculturation; Integration; Intergroup relations; Multiculturalism; Separation 1. Introduction Australia has become one of the most culturally diverse nations in the world. One can hardly walk along a major street of a city for ten minutes without passing a Chinese restaurant, or a Vietnamese groceries store or an Italian deli shop. The most significant contributor to this multicultural environment is the ever increasing levels of immigration Tel.: +61 7 3365 3070; fax: +61 7 3365 1377. E-mail address: s.liu1@uq.edu.au 0147-1767/$ - see front matter r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2007.08.003

762 ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 (Department of Immigration and Multicultural Affairs, 2007a). As Castles (1992) pointed out, nowhere is this more apparent than in a country like Australia where immigration has always been a central part of nation building (p. 549). Since the end of World War II, around 6.5 million migrants have come to Australia as settlers (Department of Immigration and Multicultural Affairs, 2007c) and this trend is increasing. In the immediate post war period only 10% of Australia s population was born overseas (Marden & Mercer, 1998). Today, nearly 25% of the population is born overseas and approximately 200 languages are spoken in the country (Department of Immigration and Multicultural Affairs, 2007b). With each encounter of alternative food, clothing, architecture, life style, art, language and medicine, we learn some new things outside of our village culture. Indeed, living with others we are being multiculturalised every day. However, behind various visible symbols of cultural diversity is a complex concept of multiculturalism (MC). At a descriptive level, MC can be used to characterise a society with diverse cultures; as an attitude, it can refer to a society s tolerance towards diversity and acceptance of equal societal participation. The question is to what extent can immigrants maintain their access to ethnic language, religion, customs and traditions, and various ethnic organisations without posing a threat to the overall political unity of the host society (Rex, 1995)? In the wake of fully maximising the national benefits of cultural diversity, we are aware of some potential threats to our cultural uniqueness. The host nationals have been expressing concerns over the threat ethnic cultures might pose on the mainstream cultural values, the political and economic power structure and the distribution of employment opportunities (e.g. Berry & Kalin, 1995; Berry, Kalin, & Taylor, 1977; Stephan, Renfro, Esses, Stephan, & Martin, 2005). The migrants, on the other hand, have been forming state and national associations to maintain their ethnic culture heritage and promote the survival of their languages within the mainstream institutions since 1973 (Department of Immigration and Multicultural Affairs, 2007a). The widely reported Cronulla riots between Anglo-Australians and Lebanese in Sydney in 2005 is just one example of the unresolved issue of maintaining cultural identity. Does MC pose a threat to our cultural identity? Do host nationals and immigrants view MC as equally beneficial? What do immigrants believe that they should do to acculturate into the host country? Is this understanding shared by the host nationals? Despite the popularity of the term in the public rhetoric, our understanding of what MC means and how our interpretation subsequently influences our acculturation strategies is still far from complete. Hence, research that enhances a deeper level of understanding of MC has important social and policy implications. This study investigated attitudes towards MC and their influence on acculturation strategies of Anglo-Australians and Asian immigrants residing in the city of Brisbane, the third largest city of Australia. Urban rather than rural areas were chosen as sampling frame because, in Australia, the majority minority groups tension tends to be in the cities due to the concentration of immigrant stream in the major urban areas (Forrest & Dunn, 2007). Anglo-Australian refers to Australians of European descent, who are considered to make up 80% of the Australian population (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2003). Asian immigrant here refers to immigrants from Northeast Asia (including China, Hong Kong and Taiwan) and Southeast Asian (including Singapore and Malaysia). Although the term Asian might be too general to capture the differences in culture uniqueness of the countries, it nevertheless provides a useful referent when drawing differences between

ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 763 Asian and Anglo-Australian groups (Leung, Pe-Pua, & Karnilowicz, 2006). Asian immigrants were selected as the prototype immigrant group because Asian community stands out as an outgroup in relation to people from the mainstream (Anlgo-Australians). Cultural distance is related to the level of acceptance or prejudice from members of the cultural majority as experienced by immigrants (Ho, Niles, Penny, & Thomas, 1994). In Australia, for example, it has become an important part of the Asian values debate, particularly in response to the increased Asian immigration and the continuing emphasis placed on Australia s role in the Asian region as being closely tied to the policy of MC (Marden & Mercer, 1998). Considering Asian immigrants constitute the largest non- English speaking migrant group (Department of Immigration and Multicultural Affairs, 2007c) and the significant contributions they continue to make to Australia s economy, business and education (DFAT, 2005), promoting harmonious interethnic relations between Asian immigrants and the Anglo-Australians is of strategic importance, culturally, politically and economically. 2. Acculturation, multiculturalism and intergroup relations The widely applied model of acculturation is developed by John Berry (e.g. Berry, 1980, 1990). According to his model, immigrants are confronted with two basic issues: the maintenance of their heritage culture and maintenance of relationships with the host society. Mapped out on this continuum, their acculturation strategies range from positive/ positive (integration), negative/positive (assimilation), positive/negative (separation) to negative/negative (marginalisation). One major conceptual shortcoming of this original model, as identified by scholars including Berry himself, is that it places the emphasis of acculturation on the minority or immigrant groups (Berry et al., 1977; Horenczyk, 1997), assuming immigrants have the freedom to pursue the acculturation strategy they prefer in the host society. However, in reality, acculturation takes place within the context of both intra- and intergroup relations; and host attitudes can exert strong influence on how immigrants experience the process (Bourhis, Moı se, Perreault, & Senécal, 1997; Kosic, Mannetti, & Sam, 2005). Like immigrants, members of a host society also develop acculturation attitudes, which define how this process should take place (Rohmann, Florack, & Piontkowski, 2006). For them, acculturation issues are whether or not they want immigrants to maintain their heritage culture and whether or not they value intergroup contact. Usually, their acculturation attitudes are referred to as integration, assimilation and separation (Bourhis et al., 1997). Results from previous research show that discordance between majority and minority acculturation attitudes could lead to ingroup bias and perceived discrimination (Zagefka & Brown, 2002). Therefore, to overcome the limitations of the original linear model, Berry (see 2005 for a review) conceptualised acculturation based on the presence of three underlying dimensions: cultural maintenance, intergroup contact and participation, and the power to decide on how to acculturate (p. 706). With the White Australia policy abolished and replaced by MC policy in the 1970s (Forrest & Dunn, 2007), immigrants are more welcome to integrate into the host culture while maintaining ties with their own ethnic heritage. The assumption that minority groups and individuals would move from their traditional way of living to a way resembling the host society, an assimilation or melting pot conception, has been gradually replaced by this multidimentional view of acculturation. Consistent with findings from studies conducted in other countries (e.g. Rohmann et al.,

764 ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 2006) research conducted in Australia shows that among immigrants, integration, which promotes cultural diversity and intergroup contact, is the most common acculturation strategy (e.g. Nesdale & Mak, 2003), followed by either assimilation or separation, while marginalisation is usually shown to be the least preferred (e.g. Liu, 2006). Evidence from previous research also indicates that integration strategy is linked with optimal psychological adjustment of immigrants (Pfafferott & Brown, 2006) and with more favourable intergroup attitudes (Zagefka & Brown, 2002). Nevertheless, national ethnocentrism to which Australianness is tightly linked (Dixson, 1999; Johnson, 2002) is still reflected in the country s legacy of Anglo-privilege and cultural dominance (Forrest & Dunn, 2006). Hence, there have been repeated discussions in the local media about Australian values and nationalism (e.g. Courier-Mail, September October 2006) to preserve Anglo-Australianness in the host culture. Previous research in Australia from the immigrant perspective indicates that the level of acceptance by members of the cultural majority decreases with cultural dissimilarity (e.g.; Nesdale, 2002). Evidence from other studies also suggests that physical dissimilarity militates against acceptance (Liebkind & Jasinskaja-Lahti, 2000). From the perspective of intergroup relations, people are more attracted to individuals who are similar to themselves than to those who are different (Duck & Barnes, 1992). Therefore, two dominant and competing acculturation strategies seem to exist: integration and separation, on which our present study would focus. Based on the literature of immigrants preferred acculturation strategy and the impact of cultural distance, we hypothesise: H1. Asian group s ratings on integration are higher than that of Anglo-Australian group; but their ratings on separation is lower than that of Anglo-Australian group. Given that attitude favoured by the mainstreamers may not necessarily be perceived in the same way by members of the minority group, MC, that stresses the importance of recognising cultural diversity within the same political framework as well as equal opportunities (Verkuyten & Martinovic, 2006), could also lead to group distinctions (Brewer, 1997) and threaten social cohesion (Berry, 2001), if interpreted differently. Berry and Kalin (1995) argue that groups are more in favour of MC when they see advantages for themselves. The ideological asymmetry hypothesis proposed by social dominance theory (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999) suggests that hierarchy-attenuating ideologies such as MC appeals more to low status groups than to high status groups because the status hierarchy is differentially beneficial for members of low and high status groups. For minority groups, presumably in the lower social status group, MC offers the possibility of maintaining their own culture and obtaining higher social status in society. The majority group members, on the other hand, may see ethnic minorities and their desire to maintain their own culture as a threat to the mainstream cultural identity and higher status position. Thus, we hypothesise: H2. Asian group s ratings on MC as a benefit are higher than that of Anglo-Australian group; but their ratings on MC as a threat are lower than that of Anglo-Australian group. A significant amount of research suggests that perception of threat plays an important role in prejudice towards outgroups in general and immigrants in particular (Berry et al., 1977; Stephan et al., 2005). The integrated threat theory identifies four domains of threat. Realistic threat concerns threat to the political and economic power of the ingroup, as well as threat to the well-being of the ingroup (Ashmore & Del Boca, 1976). Immigrants would

ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 765 seem likely to evoke such threat as they always need jobs and they may require additional resources from the host society. Symbolic threat concerns group differences in values, beliefs, morals and attitudes. Esses, Haddock, and Zanna (1993) have found that symbolic beliefs are related to Canadians prejudice toward different outgroups. Negative stereotypes, as a third domain of threat, serve as a basis for negative expectations concerning the behaviour of members of the stereotyped group. For example, when migrant group (outgroup) members are perceived to be untrustworthy, the mainstreamer group (ingroup) members may feel threatened by interacting with them. The fourth type of threat concerns intergroup anxiety. Several theorists argue that people feel personally threatened in intergroup interactions because they are concerned about negative outcomes for the self, such as being embarrassed, rejected, or ridiculed (Gudykunst, 1995; Stephan & Stephan, 1985). Interacting with immigrants is often difficult for people from the host culture due to differences in language and cultural values, and this adds intergroup anxiety in interaction. Based on the literature, we hypothesise: H3. The tendency to view MC as a benefit is positively related to integration whereas the tendency to view MC as a threat is positively related to separation; these associations hold true for both Asian and Anglo-Australian groups. 3. Method 3.1. Participants Purposive and snowball sampling technique was employed to obtain respondents. Asian immigrants, primarily from mainland China, Hong Kong, Taiwan, Singapore and Malaysia, were contacted through Asian community associations, churches and students to obtain initial consent in taking part in the survey. One common characteristic feature among participants from different Asian subgroups was they all had a Chinese origin (e.g. either one or both parents were Chinese). The Anglo-Australians were contacted with the help of colleagues and friends. Approximately 430 questionnaires were distributed and eventually, 241 (133 Asians and 108 Anglo-Australians) respondents from nine suburbs of Brisbane voluntarily participated in the survey, making a response rate of 56%. The pool of respondents consisted of 44% males and 56% females; their age ranged from under 20 to 60 with half of them (51%) falling between 21 and 30; approximately 41% of the respondents received undergraduate education; 54% of the respondents were full-time students and 28% of them were working full time; 61% of the respondents were at the lower end of annual income (at or under A$20,000 per annum) and 9% were at the higher end (at or above A$60,000 per annum); 49% of the participants resided in areas moderately populated by immigrants, the rest were divided between living in suburbs either lightly populated (39%) or densely populated (12%) by immigrants; all Australian respondents were born in Australia whereas only 10% of Asian respondents were born in Australia. 3.2. Instrument The questionnaire consisted of 37 items categorised into three sections: attitudes toward MC, acculturation strategies, and demographic data. Multicultural attitudes were

766 ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 measured on two scales: MC as a threat and MC as a benefit. Threat consisted of 8 items concerning cultural diversity as a threat to national unity, safety of neighbourhood and host cultural values (e.g. The unity of Australia is weakened by ethnic minorities of different cultural background sticking to their old ways). Benefit was measured by 10 items concerning cultural diversity as a benefit to Australian society and the necessity of encouraging cultural awareness in schools, workplace and neighbourhood as well as equal societal participation (e.g. It is good for Australia to have different groups with a distinct cultural background living in this country). Acculturation strategies comprised of two domains: integration and separation. Integration consisted of 7 items encouraging intergroup interactions (e.g. Anglo-Australians and ethnic minorities should seek more contact with one another). Separation consisted of 5 items expressing reluctance to interact with ethnic minorities at work, in public places or neighbourhood (e.g. I don t like being in a bus or on a train in which there are ethnic minorities). The 30 items measuring multicultural attitudes (benefit or threat) and acculturation strategies (integration or separation) were adapted from the Multicultural Attitude Scale (Breugemanns & Van de Vijver, 2004), most of them, however, came from the survey instrument originally developed by Berry et al. (1977). The items were formulated as statements, followed by a 5-point Likert scale with 1 standing for strongly disagree and 5 representing strongly agree (see Appendix for a complete survey questionnaire). The overall alpha coefficients of the four scales ranged from.74 to.83; and the reliability scores across the two groups of participants ranged from.62 to.85, similar to the scores of scales obtained in Leung et al. s study (2006) examining differences between Anglo-Celtic and three Asian groups on psychological adaptation. One item measuring separation was deleted to achieve better internal scale consistency. The remaining 7 questions in the survey intended to collect demographic data such as gender, place of birth, age, education, employment status, income and residence. Mean scores of each scale (total score divided by the number of items) were calculated and each scale was treated as a variable in subsequent data analysis. Table 1 summarises the reliability scores of the scales. 3.3. Procedures A cover letter that outlined the nature and purpose of the study, information on the responsible researcher/institution and instruction on how to fill out the survey accompanied each questionnaire. Questionnaires were distributed via the researcher, gate keepers of Asian immigrant associations, research assistants and the mail. In order to protect anonymity of participants, a pre-paid return envelope was attached for each Table 1 Reliability coefficients of the variables Variable Asians (n ¼ 133) Australians (n ¼ 108) Overall (n ¼ 241) MC as a threat.72.84.80 MC as benefit.74.85.83 Integration.62.74.74 Separation.66.80.74

ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 767 respondent to mail the completed questionnaire back to the researcher, regardless of how the questionnaire was distributed to the participant. 4. Results The mean and standard deviation scores of the two groups of respondents on all four variables/scales are summarised in Table 2. H1 predicted that Asians rated integration higher but separation lower as compared to Anglo-Australians. Results from independent sample t-test indicated that Asians ratings on integration were significantly higher than that of Australians (t ¼ 6.31, po.001). This finding was consistent with results from previous studies that identified integration as the most preferred acculturation strategy by immigrants (e.g. Bourhis et al., 1997; Pfafferott & Brown, 2006). However, the difference between the two groups in their ratings on separation did not reach significance level (t ¼ 1.70, n.s.). Hence, H1 was partially confirmed. While the desire to integrate into the host society was stronger in minority groups (Asians), the tendency to separate either by sticking to their ethnic cultural heritage (Asians) or by holding on to their Anglo-Australian values (Australians) was similar in strength in both groups of respondents. H2 hypothesised that Asian immigrant group s ratings on MC as a benefit were higher but their ratings on MC as a threat were lower than that of Anglo-Australian group. Results from independent sample t-test supported this hypothesis. Asians tended to view MC more as a benefit (t ¼ 6.54, po.001) but less as a threat (t ¼ 3.36, po.01) as compared to Australians. These findings supported the assumption of the ideological asymmetry hypothesis that proposed MC would appeal more to minority or low status groups (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). These findings also provided some support for the integrated threat theory that assumes the host society could perceive migrants as a threat to the mainstream culture (e.g. Esses et al., 1993). H3 predicted that viewing MC as a benefit was positively related to integration whereas viewing MC as a threat was positively related to separation; and these associations would hold true for both Asians and Anglo-Australians. This hypothesis was, again, only partially supported. To test the between group differences (Asians and Australians) on acculturation strategies (integration and separation) at different levels of attitudes towards MC, the original two continuous variables measuring attitudes towards MC (MC as a threat and MC as a benefit) were recoded into 3-level categorical variables (High, Mid and Table 2 Mean and standard deviation scores of Asian and Anglo-Australian groups on the variables Variable Asians (n ¼ 133) Australians (n ¼ 108) Overall (n ¼ 241) Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD MC as a threat 2.83.55 3.13.80 2.96.69 MC as benefit 3.96.41 3.53.41 3.78.55 Integration 3.65.46 3.21.61 3.46.57 Separation 2.20.67 2.37.91 2.51.68 Note: Variables are measured on 5 point Likert scale with 1 standing for strongly disagree and 5 standing for strongly agree.

768 ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 Table 3 Means (and standard deviations) for integration MC as threat Asian Australian Low 3.72 (.47) 3.59 (.52) Mid 3.55 (.36) 3.08 (.46) High 3.25 (.91) 2.71 (.56) Note. Higher scores indicate more in favour of integration. Low) and used as the first independent variable. Ethnicity was treated as the second independent variable. A 3 (MC as threat Low, Mid, or High) 2 (Ethnicity Asian or Australian) ANOVA was performed to examine the main effect of each independent variable as well as the interactions of the two independent variables on the dependent variable (e.g. integration). Similar tests were run with MC as a benefit and separation as acculturation strategy. The first ANOVA tested the effect of MC as a threat on integration. Levene s test indicated that the homogeneity of variance assumption was not violated (p4.05). Results revealed a significant main effect of MC as a threat on integration, F(2, 233) ¼ 20.19, po.001. This main effect showed that attitudes towards MC significantly influenced acculturation strategies. Post-Hoc Tests (Scheffe) revealed that significant differences existed between people in low and mid threat groups and between people in mid and high threat groups with the tendency of higher scores on MC as a threat leading to lower scores on integration. Ethnicity was also found to have a significant main effect on integration, F(1, 233) ¼ 15.15, po.001, with Asians scoring significantly higher on integration than Australians (see H1). In addition, results also showed a significant interaction between MC as a threat and ethnicity, F(2, 233) ¼ 3.61, po.05. Thus, the influence of viewing MC as a threat on integration depended on the ethnicity of the respondents (see Table 3). Fig. 1 showed that for Australians viewing MC as a threat was more strongly associated with integration as compared to Asians, although the association for both groups was in the negative direction (see Fig. 1). The second ANOVA tested the effect of MC as a threat on separation. Levene s test indicated that the homogeneity of variance assumption was not violated (p4.05). Results revealed a significant main effect of MC as a threat on separation, F(2, 237) ¼ 55.98, po.001. Post Hoc tests (Scheffe) revealed that significant differences existed between people in low and mid threat groups and between people in mid and high threat groups with the tendency of higher scores on MC as a threat leading to higher scores on separation. Ethnicity was also found to have a significant main effect on separation, F(1, 237) ¼ 4.09, po.05, with Asians in general scoring significantly lower on separation than Australians (see H1). In addition, results also showed a significant interaction between MC as a threat and ethnicity, F(2, 237) ¼ 8.38, po.001. Thus, the influence of viewing MC as a threat on separation depended on the ethnicity of the respondents (see Table 4). An interesting finding in the Asian group was that respondents in mid threat group scored highest on separation. This result indicated that the link between attitudes towards MC and acculturation strategies was viewed differently by respondents from the host culture and those from the ethnic culture. Post-Hoc multiple comparisons of Australian respondents showed significant differences between low and mid threat group and between

ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 769 Asian Ethnicity Australian 3.75 3.50 Integration 3.25 3.00 2.75 Low Mid MC as a threat High Fig. 1. Interaction on integration. Table 4 Means (and standard deviations) for separation MC as threat Asian Australian Low 1.92 (.56) 1.67 (.59) Mid 2.64 (.62) 2.50 (.55) High 2.37 (.43) 3.49 (.60) Note. Higher scores indicate more in favour of separation. mid and high threat group, with higher ratings on MC as a threat relating consistently to higher ratings on separation. However, results from multiple comparisons of Asian respondents only revealed mid threat group s significantly higher ratings on separation as compared to low threat group. No significant difference was found between high and low threat groups, or between high and mid threat groups. Fig. 2 showed a positive linear relationship between viewing MC as a threat and separation for the Australian respondents. However, for Asian respondents, this association tended to be curvilinear (see Fig. 2). Results from the third and fourth ANOVA revealed a main effect of MC as a benefit on both integration, F(2, 234) ¼ 47.13, po.001; and on separation, F(2, 238) ¼ 39.63, po.001. Neither the main effect of ethnicity nor interaction reached significance level. Thus, the influence of MC as a benefit on acculturation strategies did not depend on the ethnicity of the respondents. Post-Hoc multiple comparisons indicated significant differences between high and mid benefit groups and between mid and low benefit groups,

770 ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 Asian Ethnicity Australian 3.50 3.00 Separation 2.50 2.00 1.50 Low Mid MC as a threat High Fig. 2. Interaction on separation. with higher ratings of MC as a benefit positively related to integration and negatively related to separation. 5. Discussion and implications The arrival of immigrants as new settlers may bring various changes to the host cultural environment. As pointed out by Sayegh and Lasry (1993), it is difficult to imagine a host society which would not be transformed after immigrants have been accepted as full participants into the social and institutional networks of that society (p. 99). Thus, not only the immigrant group but also the host nationals are undergoing psychological and sociological adjustment as a result of the presence of culturally distinctive others (Searle & Ward, 1990; Ward & Kennedy, 2001). Under some circumstances, the psychological adjustment for mainstreamers might seem even more difficult as compared to that experienced by immigrants. The reason is that immigrants in many cases are aware of the need to adjust to their host cultural environment at the time, if not well before, they set their foot in the host country. The mainstreamers, however, might not be equally wellprepared to accept or adjust to various changes in their lives brought about by the immigrant population. Hence, in studying MC and acculturation, it is important to take into consideration both the ethnic minority and the majority group members because a lack of reciprocal attitudes may hamper the realisation of a positively diverse and equal society. Consistent with findings from previous studies (e.g. Piontkowski, Florack, Hoelker, & Obdrza lek, 2000; Rohmann et al., 2006; Zagefka & Brown, 2002), this study has identified integration as the most preferred acculturation strategy by Asian respondents. Integration offers immigrants the opportunity to keep their ethnic cultural practice while maintaining

ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 771 a positive relationship with the host society. Integration probably benefits immigrants more as it, among other things, provides them an opportunity to upgrade their group s lower social status. An important assumption of social identity theory is that membership in a high-status group is desirable because it may contribute to positive social identity (Ellemers, Wilke, & van Knippenberg, 1993). To maintain a positive self-concept derived from a satisfying social identity, individuals who belong to a group of subordinate status may either strive for a higher status by leaving their low status group or try to upgrade the status position of their group as a whole (Tajfel, 1978). In the case of immigrants, it is very difficult, if not impossible, for them to upgrade the status position of their ethnic group as a whole, as they came to the host country from different walks of life and for various reasons. Therefore, efforts to achieve a positive social identity are focused on integrating into the host group, rather than remaining as a member of the stranger (outgroup). Evidence from previous research suggests that integration is associated to positive acculturation outcomes (see Berry, 2005 for a review). However, the question is whether or not our society has provided immigrants with an environment in which they feel welcome to integrate into their host society. Findings from Nesdale and Mak s study (2003) on the relative significance of a range of variables in predicting the ethnic identification of immigrants in Australia show that increasing cultural distance encourages immigrants to remain psychologically located within their ethnic groups (e.g. immigrants from Hong Kong as compared to those from New Zealand). Hence, the extent to which the mainstreamers allow members of ethnic groups to maintain their own culture and partake in relationships with the dominant cultural group plays an important role in the construction of a truly multicultural society (Breugemanns & Van de Vijver, 2004). Results from our study have revealed Asian group s significant higher ratings on integration as compared to that of the Australian group. Hence, promoting intergroup understanding is a major challenge facing both the host nationals and the immigrants as differences in acculturation orientations may result in problematic or conflictual intergroup relations (e.g. Berry, 2006; Bourhis et al., 1997). Results from this study show that Asian immigrant group tend to view MC more as a benefit as compared to the Australian group that viewed MC more as a threat. This finding provides support for the theoretical assumption that MC is typically seen as having more to offer to minority groups than to the majority group (Verkuyten & Martinovic, 2006). Explained from the perspective of the social dominance theory, MC is differentially beneficial for lower status and higher status groups. For the mainstreamers, MC could pose a threat to their cultural dominance and existing higher social status (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). According to the multicultural hypothesis (Berry et al., 1977), confidence in one s cultural identity involves a sense of security which is a psychological precondition for the acceptance of those who are culturally different. Conversely, when people feel their cultural identity is threatened, they will reject others. The implication for policy makers is to create a sense of confidence in different cultural groups, including the host nationals so as to reduce discriminatory attitudes (Berry, 2006). The presence of visible multicultural symbols (e.g. ethnic shops) alone is not necessarily an indicator of a true multicultural society unless there are mutual acceptance and equal societal participation of all cultural groups. An interesting finding in this study is the higher rating on separation strategy by Asian respondents who view MC as a moderate threat. From the perspective of group vitality theory, the stronger the vitality of immigrant groups, the more likely such groups are to

772 ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 adopt orientations that reflect their own priorities rather than those determined by the host majority (Bourhis et al., 1997; Giles, Bourhis, & Taylor, 1977). Asian immigrant groups are considered as having relatively strong vitality given the size of their community and their significant contribution to Australian economy, education and business sectors. They also stand out as an outgroup due to their recognisable physical appearance as compared to Anglo-Australians. When threat level is perceived as moderate, Asian immigrants are more likely to act as a distinctive and collective entity within the host society, leaning towards a separation orientation. However, when the perceived threat level is low, the need to cling tightly on to their own ethnic group for moral support might not be as strong due to less pressure from the host society. On the other hand, being in a presumably low status group, when the perceived threat level is high, they might try to assimilate to the host society due to more pressure from the host society. Australians, as host nationals in this study, do not necessarily experience similar pressures, as their choice of acculturation strategies is based on the extent to which they would like to accept the minority groups into their society. Thus, for the Australians, the association between attitudes towards MC (threat or a benefit) and acculturation strategies (integrate or separate) seems to be a more linear relationship. 6. Conclusion, limitations and further study The present study further supports the argument that the processes of immigrants adjustment involve adaptation on the part of both the immigrants and the receiving population (e.g. Berry et al., 1977; Rohmann et al., 2006). However, based on our data, we could not draw causal conclusions about whether attitudes towards MC influence acculturation strategies or vice versa. Such causal relationships need to be carefully studied if one is to implement any form of intervention (Kosic et al., 2005). Future research might use probability sampling and a larger sample size that permits more rigorous multivariate analysis to draw conclusions about the causal relations between attitudes toward MC and acculturation strategies. Future study may also take into consideration how the majority acculturation attitudes are perceived by the minority groups and how this perceived attitudes influence minority acculturation orientations. Our study relied on survey as the source of data. While quantitative data presents a general pattern of the relationship, it does not thoroughly explore the factors underpinning discordance in attitudes and behaviours between Asians and Australians. Further study may investigate the underlying factors of the different attitudes and strategies by conducting in-depth interviews. Survey data combined with findings from interviews would help us to get a more comprehensive understanding of the formation of those attitudes, in addition to the self-reported attitudes and behaviours. In addition, future research could use longitudinal surveys to monitor the change in attitudes and behaviours over time as well as to capture how cultural identities might change in different contexts. The Asian respondents in this study included people from northeast and southeast Asia such as Hong Kong, mainland China, Singapore, Taiwan and Malaysia. Differences in attitudes towards MC and acculturation strategies may exist across respondents from different countries due to differences in political and social systems. Future study may compare across the different Asian groups to further our understanding of the association between attitudes and strategies. Future study may also take into consideration the

ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 773 motivation or reason for immigrants to come to the host country, as they are likely to influence the desire to integrate into the host country. Finally, our data was collected from urban areas. We assume differences would exist between regional and urban areas as geographical locations may affect people s access to information and opportunities to engage in interethnic contact. Future study may use cluster sampling strategy to obtain samples from different cities and regions of Australia and compare people s ratings across geographical locations. Such studies could generate a more comprehensive picture of the public s endorsement of MC and have implications for policy making regarding promoting cultural diversity and interethnic relations. Appendix. Survey on multiculturalism and acculturation in Australia Instructions: Please circle the choice that best describes your attitudes. Thank you for your time and participation in the survey! Items 1. It is good for Australia to have different groups with a distinct cultural background living in this country. a Strongly Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly agree disagree 2. I do not like being in a bus or train in which there are ethnic minorities. d 3. The unity of Australia is weakened by ethnic minorities of different cultural backgrounds sticking to their old ways. b 4. Districts with many ethnic minorities are less safe. b 5. Too many ethnic minorities are living in Australia. b 6. It is best for Australia that ethnic minorities keep their culture and customs. c 7. I feel at ease when I am in a district with many ethnic minorities. c 8. Ethnic minorities are familiar with Australian culture and customs. c

774 ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 9. I feel uneasy when ethnic minorities talk to one another in a language I do not understand. d 10. Ethnic minorities in Australia put sufficient effort into getting a job. a 11. Ethnic minorities are living in suburbs densely populated by immigrants. e 12. Ethnic minorities should learn to speak proper English. b 13. I approve of ethnic minorities wearing their traditional clothes. a 14. I dislike it when an ethnic minority person does not understand me. b 15. People who come to live in Australia should change their behaviours to be more like Australians. b 16. Ethnic minority parents should encourage their children to retain the culture and traditions of their homeland. a 17. Anglo-Australians should learn more about the culture and customs of difference cultural groups. a 18. I would rather live next to an Anglo-Australian family than next to an ethnic minority family. b 19. Australian companies should put more effort into hiring ethnic minorities. a 20. Australian schools should think more about the cultural background of their pupils. c 21. Police should patrol more in districts with many ethnic minorities. b

ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 775 22. Anglo-Australians should support ethnic minorities more in the preservation of their culture and customs in Australia. a 23. Anglo-Australians and ethnic minorities should cooperate more to solve problems in Australia. a 24. I do not like being attended to in a shop by an ethnic minority person. d 25. Anglo-Australians and ethnic minorities should seek more contact with one another. c 26. Anglo-Australian children should have both native and ethnic minority teachers. c 27. More ethnic minority people should work in Australian companies. a 28. Australian children should play more with ethnic minority children. c 29. I would not like an ethnic minority boss at work. d 30. Anglo-Australians and ethnic minorities should have equal rights. a Note: a MC as benefit; b MC as threat; c Integration; d Separation; e Deleted item. Please tell us something about yourself: Gender: 1. Male 2. Female Age: 1. o ¼ 20 2. 21 30 3. 31 40 4. 41 50 5. 51 60 6. 4 ¼ 61 Place of birth: 1. Australia 2. Overseas Highest formal education completed: 1. High school 2. Tertiary college 3. Undergraduate 4. Postgraduate

776 ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 Are you currently: 1. Studying full time 2. Studying part time and working part time 3. Working full time 4. Unemployed 5. Other (please specify ) Your annual income is: 1. o ¼ A$20,000 2. A$21,000 40,000 3. A$41,000 60,000 4. A$61,000 80,000 5. o ¼ A$81,000 The suburb where you live is: 1. Lightly populated by ethnic minorities 2. Moderately populated by ethnic minorities 3. Densely populated by ethnic minorities References Ashmore, R. D., & Del Boca, F. K. (1976). Psychological approaches to understanding intergroup conflict. In R. A. Katz (Ed.), Toward the elimination of racism (pp. 73 124). New York: Pergamon. Australian Bureau of Statistics (2003). Population characteristics: Ancestry of Australia s population. Australian Social Trends. Retrieved 20 August 2007 from /http://www.abs.gov.aus. Berry, J. (1980). Acculturation as varieties of adaptation. In A. Padilla (Ed.), Acculturation: Theory, models and some new findings (pp. 9 25). Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Berry, J. (1990). Psychology of acculturation: Understanding individuals moving between cultures. In R. Brislin (Ed.), Applied cross-cultural psychology (pp. 232 253). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Berry, J. (2001). A psychology of immigration. Journal of Social Issues, 57, 615 631. Berry, J. (2005). Acculturation: Living successfully in two cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29, 697 712. Berry, J. (2006). Mutual attitudes among immigrants and ethnocultural groups in Canada. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30, 719 734. Berry, J., & Kalin, R. (1995). Multicultural and ethnic attitudes in Canada: Overview of the 1991 survey. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 27, 301 320. Berry, J., Kalin, R., & Taylor, D. (1977). Multiculturalism and ethnic attitudes in Canada. Ottawa: Ministry of Supply and Services. Bourhis, R. Y., Moïse, L. C., Perreault, S., & Senécal, S. (1997). Towards an interactive acculturation model: A social psychological approach. International Journal of Psychology, 32(6), 369 386. Breugemanns, S. M., & Van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2004). Antecedents and components of majority attitudes toward multiculturalism in the Netherlands. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 53(3), 400 422. Brewer, M. B. (1997). The social psychology of intergroup relations: Can research inform practice? Journal of Social Issues, 53, 197 211. Castles, S. (1992). The Australian model of immigration and multiculturalism: Is it applicable to Europe? International Immigration Review, 26(2), 549 567.

ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 777 Department of Foreign Affairs and Trade (DFAT). (2005). Unlocking China s services sector. Canberra: Commonwealth of Australia. Department of Immigration and Multicultural Affairs. (2007a). Fact sheet 6: The evolution of Australia s multicultural policy. Retrieved 19 January 2007 from /http://www.dimia.gov.au/media/fact-sheets/06evolution.htmls. Department of Immigration and Multicultural Affairs. (2007b). Fact sheet 8: Abolition of the White Australia policy. Retrieved 19 January 2007 from /http://www.dimia.gov.au/media/fact-sheets/08abolition.htmls. Department of Immigration and Multicultural Affairs. (2007c). Fact sheet 4: More than 60 years of post war migration. Retrieved 19 January 2007 from /http://www.dimia.gov.au/media/fact-sheets/04fifty.htmls. Dixson, M. (1999). The imaginary Australian: Anglo-Celts and identity 1788 to present. Sydney: University of New South Wales Press. Duck, S., & Barnes, M. K. (1992). Disagreeing about agreement: Reconciling differences about similarity. Communication Monographs, 59, 199 208. Ellemers, N., Wilke, H., & van Knippenberg, A. (1993). Effects of the legitimacy of low group of individual status on individual and collective status-enhancement strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(5), 766 778. Esses, V. M., Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1993). Values, stereotypes, and emotions as determinants of intergroup attitudes. In D. M. Mackie, & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition and stereotyping (pp. 137 166). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Forrest, J., & Dunn, K. (2006). Racism and intolerance in Eastern Australia: A geographic perspective. Australian Geographer, 37, 167 186. Forrest, J., & Dunn, K. (2007). Constructing racism in Sydney, Australia s largest ethnicity. Urban Studies, 44(4), 699 721. Giles, H., Bourhis, R. Y., & Taylor, D. M. (1977). Towards a theory of language in ethnic group relations. In H. Giles (Ed.), Language, ethnicity and intergroup relations. London: Academic Press. Gudykunst, W. B. (1995). Anxiety/uncertainty management (AUM) theory: Development and current status. In R. L. Wiseman (Ed.), Intercultural communication theory (pp. 8 51). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Ho, R., Niles, S., Penny, R., & Thomas, A. (1994). Migrants and multiculturalism A survey of attitudes in Darwin. Australian Psychologist, 29, 62 70. Horenczyk, G. (1997). Immigrants perceptions of host attitudes and other reconstruction of cultural groups. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46, 35 38. Johnson, C. (2002). The dilemmas of ethnic privilege: A comparison of constructions of British, English and Anglo-Celtic identity in contemporary British and Australian political discourse. Ethnicities, 2, 163 188. Kosic, A., Mannetti, L., & Sam, D. L. (2005). The role of majority attitudes towards out-group in the perception of the acculturation strategies of immigrants. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29, 273 288. Leung, C., Pe-Pua, R., & Karnilowicz, W. (2006). Psychological adaptation and autonomy among adolescents in Australia: A comparison of Anglo-Celtic and three Asian groups. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30(1), 99 118. Liebkind, K., & Jasinskaja-Lahti, I. (2000). The influence of experiences of discrimination on psychological stress: A comparison of seven immigrant groups. Journal of Community and Applied Social psychology, 10, 1 16. Liu, S. (2006). An examination of the effects of print media exposure and contact on subjective social reality and acculturation attitudes. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30, 365 382. Marden, P., & Mercer, D. (1998). Locating strangers: Multiculturalism, citizenship and nationhood in Australia. Political Geography, 17(8), 939 958. Nesdale, D. (2002). Acculturation attitudes and the ethnic and host-country identification of immigrants. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(7), 1488 1507. Nesdale, D., & Mak, A. S. (2003). Ethnic identification, self-esteem and immigrant psychological health. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 27(1), 23 40. Pfafferott, I., & Brown, R. (2006). Acculturation preferences of majority and minority adolescents in Germany in the context of society and family. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30, 703 717. Piontkowski, U., Florack, A., Hoelker, P., & Obdrzálek, P. (2000). Predicting acculturation attitudes of dominant and non-dominant groups. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 24, 1 26. Rex, J. (1995). Ethnic identity and the nation state: The political sociology of multi-cultural societies. Social Identities, 1(1), 21 34.

778 ARTICLE IN PRESS S. Liu / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 31 (2007) 761 778 Rohmann, A., Florack, A., & Piontkowski, U. (2006). The role of discordant acculturation attitudes in perceived threat: An analysis of host and immigrant attitudes in Germany. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30, 683 702. Sayegh, L., & Lasry, J. C. (1993). Immigrants adaptation in Canada: Assimilation, acculturation, and orthogonal cultural identification. Canadian Psychology, 34, 98 109. Searle, W., & Ward, C. (1990). The prediction of psychological and sociological adjustment during cross-cultural transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 14(4), 449 464. Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Stephan, W. G., Renfro, C. L., Esses, V. M., Stephan, C. W., & Martin, T. (2005). The effects of feeling threatened on attitudes toward immigrants. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29, 1 19. Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journal of Social issues, 41, 157 175. Tajfel, H. (1978). The social psychology of minorities. New York: Minority Rights Group. Verkuyten, M., & Martinovic, B. (2006). Understanding multicultural attitudes: The role of group status, identification, friendships, and justifying ideologies. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30, 1 18. Ward, D., & Kennedy, A. (2001). Coping with cross-cultural transition. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 32(5), 636 642. Zagefka, H., & Brown, R. (2002). The relationship between acculturation strategies, relative fit and intergroup relations: Immigrant majority relations in Germany. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 171 188.