Ohio State University

Similar documents
Report on Citizen Opinions about Voting & Elections

Toplines. UMass Amherst/WBZ Poll of MA Likely Primary Voters

Toplines. UMass Amherst/WBZ Poll of NH Likely Voters

Toplines. UMass Amherst/WBZ Poll of NH Likely Voters

Likely General Election Voter Survey

Toplines. UMass Amherst/WBZ Poll of MA Registered/Likely Voters

Nevada Poll Results Tarkanian 39%, Heller 31% (31% undecided) 31% would renominate Heller (51% want someone else, 18% undecided)

Toplines. UMass Amherst/WBZ Poll of MA Registered/Likely Voters

Illustrating voter behavior and sentiments of registered Muslim voters in the swing states of Florida, Michigan, Ohio, Pennsylvania, and Virginia.

Toplines. UMass Amherst/WBZ Poll of NH Likely Primary Voters

November 2018 Hidden Tribes: Midterms Report

1. A Republican edge in terms of self-described interest in the election. 2. Lower levels of self-described interest among younger and Latino

Who Votes Without Identification? Using Affidavits from Michigan to Learn About the Potential Impact of Strict Photo Voter Identification Laws

Consolidating Democrats The strategy that gives a governing majority

National Survey Examines Marriage, Family, Immigration, Health care and Technology in the Age of Trump

For immediate release Monday, March 7 Contact: Dan Cassino ;

Case Study: Get out the Vote

Survey Overview. Survey date = September 29 October 1, Sample Size = 780 likely voters. Margin of Error = ± 3.51% Confidence level = 95%

Supplementary Materials A: Figures for All 7 Surveys Figure S1-A: Distribution of Predicted Probabilities of Voting in Primary Elections

Cato Institute Policing in America Survey

OHIO: CLINTON HOLDS SMALL EDGE; PORTMAN LEADS FOR SENATE

Trump Topple: Which Trump Supporters Are Disapproving of the President s Job Performance?

Partisan Nation: The Rise of Affective Partisan Polarization in the American Electorate

POLL: CLINTON MAINTAINS BIG LEAD OVER TRUMP IN BAY STATE. As early voting nears, Democrat holds 32-point advantage in presidential race

Red Oak Strategic Presidential Poll

Weekly Tracking Poll Week 3: September 25-Oct 1 (MoE +/-4.4%)

Clinton Maintains 3% Lead in Michigan (Clinton 47% - Trump 44% - Johnson 4% - Stein 1%)

Clinton Lead Cut to 8% in Michigan (Clinton 49% - Trump 41%- Johnson 3% - Stein 1%)

NBC News/WSJ/Marist Poll

Young Voters in the 2010 Elections

Subject: Pinellas County Congressional Election Survey

REGISTERED VOTERS October 30, 2016 October 13, 2016 Approve Disapprove Unsure 7 6 Total

PENNSYLVANIA: SMALL GOP LEAD IN CD01

This Rising American Electorate & Working Class Strike Back

Florida Atlantic University Poll: Trump Edging Clinton in Florida; Murphy and Rubio poised for tough Senate race

Alabama Republican Presidential Primary Poll 2/26/16. None

CRUZ & KASICH RUN STRONGER AGAINST CLINTON THAN TRUMP TRUMP GOP CANDIDACY COULD FLIP MISSISSIPPI FROM RED TO BLUE

The Cook Political Report / LSU Manship School Midterm Election Poll

RBS SAMPLING FOR EFFICIENT AND ACCURATE TARGETING OF TRUE VOTERS

NBC News/WSJ/Marist Poll. April New York Questionnaire

THE EFFECT OF EARLY VOTING AND THE LENGTH OF EARLY VOTING ON VOTER TURNOUT

By David Lauter. 1 of 5 12/12/2016 9:39 AM

POLL RESULTS. Page 1 of 6

Percentages of Support for Hillary Clinton by Party ID

The Presidential Election. Paul Beck, The Ohio State University Lifelong Learning Institute December 7, 2016

Colorado 2014: Comparisons of Predicted and Actual Turnout

AP PHOTO/MATT VOLZ. Voter Trends in A Final Examination. By Rob Griffin, Ruy Teixeira, and John Halpin November 2017

The Attack of the Bots and Trolls: The Social Storms that are Destroying Public Confidence in Institutions

Statewide Survey on Job Approval of President Donald Trump

GW POLITICS POLL 2018 MIDTERM ELECTION WAVE 1

Edging toward an earthquake Report on the WVWV March National Survey

FL-15 GENERAL ELECTION OCTOBER 2018

Clinton Lead Cut in Half from August (Clinton 47% - Trump 42% in 2-way and Clinton 45% - Trump 39% in 4-way)

Heading into the Conventions: A Tied Race July 8-12, 2016

These are the highlights of the latest Field Poll completed among a random sample of 997 California registered voters.

THE WORKMEN S CIRCLE SURVEY OF AMERICAN JEWS. Jews, Economic Justice & the Vote in Steven M. Cohen and Samuel Abrams

THE FIELD POLL. UCB Contact

Iowa Voting Series, Paper 4: An Examination of Iowa Turnout Statistics Since 2000 by Party and Age Group

PENNSYLVANIA: DEMOCRATS LEAD FOR BOTH PRESIDENT AND SENATE

Clinton Leads by 13% in Michigan before Last Debate (Clinton 51% - Trump 38%- Johnson 6% - Stein 2%)

FOR RELEASE APRIL 26, 2018

STAR TRIBUNE MINNESOTA POLL. April 25-27, Presidential race

Changes in Party Identification among U.S. Adult Catholics in CARA Polls, % 48% 39% 41% 38% 30% 37% 31%

A A P I D ATA Asian American Voter Survey. Sponsored by Civic Leadership USA

The Battleground: Democratic Perspective September 7 th, 2016

Tulane University Post-Election Survey November 8-18, Executive Summary

CONGRESSIONAL CAMPAIGN EFFECTS ON CANDIDATE RECOGNITION AND EVALUATION

CLINTON NARROWLY LEADS TRUMP IN FLORIDA -- GOP THIRD PARTY DEFECTIONS & HISPANIC VOTERS CREATING THE CURRENT GAP

Loras College Statewide Wisconsin Survey October/November 2016

Battleground 2016: new game. June 30, 2016

PENNSYLVANIA: CD01 INCUMBENT POPULAR, BUT RACE IS CLOSE

Rising American Electorate & Working Class Women Strike Back. November 9, 2018

Asian American Survey

ALABAMA: TURNOUT BIG QUESTION IN SENATE RACE

PENNSYLVANIA: UNCERTAIN DEM EDGE IN CD07

Who is registered to vote in Illinois?

TREND REPORT: Like everything else in politics, the mood of the nation is highly polarized

Practice Questions for Exam #2

Eagleton Institute of Politics Rutgers University New Brunswick 191 Ryders Lane New Brunswick, New Jersey

American Dental Association

The AAPI Electorate in 2016: A Deeper Look at California

The Case of the Disappearing Bias: A 2014 Update to the Gerrymandering or Geography Debate

Experiments in Election Reform: Voter Perceptions of Campaigns Under Preferential and Plurality Voting

The lost green Conservative

Florida Republican Presidential Primary Poll 3/14/16. Fox 13 Tampa Bay Fox 35 Orlando Florida Times-Union

******DRAFT***** Muhlenberg College/Morning Call 2016 Pennsylvania Republican Presidential Primary Survey. Mid April Version

Marist College Institute for Public Opinion 2455 South Road, Poughkeepsie, NY Phone Fax

Thinking back to the Presidential Election in 2016, do you recall if you supported ROTATE FIRST TWO, or someone else?

Friends of Democracy Corps and Greenberg Quinlan Rosner Research. Stan Greenberg and James Carville, Democracy Corps

Who Votes for America s Mayors?

Law professor Larry Lessig claims that at least twenty Republican electors are considering abandoning the President-elect.

2016 Presidential Elections

Emerson College Poll: Iowa Leaning For Trump 44% to 41%. Grassley, Coasting to a Blowout, Likely to Retain Senate Seat.

Abstract for: Population Association of America 2005 Annual Meeting Philadelphia PA March 31 to April 2

The GOP Civil War & Its Opportunities Report from Republican Party Project Survey

Submission to the Speaker s Digital Democracy Commission

Lab 3: Logistic regression models

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, December, 2016, Low Approval of Trump s Transition but Outlook for His Presidency Improves

POLL RESULTS. Question 1: Do you approve or disapprove of the job performance of President Donald Trump? Approve 46% Disapprove 44% Undecided 10%

Presidential Race. Virginia Illinois Maine. Published Nov 1 Oct 13 Nov 1 Sept 22 Oct 31 Sept 7. Hillary Clinton 49% 46% 53% 45% 46% 44%

Transcription:

Fake News Did Have a Significant Impact on the Vote in the 2016 Election: Original Full-Length Version with Methodological Appendix By Richard Gunther, Paul A. Beck, and Erik C. Nisbet Ohio State University It is now clear that social media and intensely partisan television and radio broadcasts disseminated a massive number of messages during the 2016 Presidential election campaign designed to demonize candidates and seriously distort the facts upon which many voters might base their electoral choices. One recent study of nearly 25,000 social media messages circulated in the key battleground state of Michigan reported that when the amount of junk news is added to the number of links to unverified WikiLeaks content and Russian-origin news stories, it appears that fully 46.5% of all the content that is presented as news and information about politics and the election is of an untrustworthy provenance or falls under the definition of propaganda based on its use of language and emotional appeals. 1 What is not clear, however, is how much of an impact, if any, these false news items had on the outcome of the election. To our knowledge, there have been no empirically based studies that have systematically assessed the extent to which believing fake news stories actually influenced voting decisions in 2016. In December and January 2016/17, we undertook a nationwide internet survey (conducted by YouGov) which enables us to estimate that impact. Our analysis leads us to the conclusion that fake news most likely did have a substantial impact on the voting decisions of a strategically important set of voters those who voted for Barack Obama in 2012. Indeed, given the very narrow margins of victory by Donald Trump in key battleground states, this impact may have been sufficient to deprive Hillary Clinton of a victory in the Electoral College. We focus our analysis on the 2016 electoral behavior of 585 respondents (of a total sample of 1,600) who had voted for Barack Obama in 2012. This strategic subset of the electorate was selected for two reasons. The first is that, if Hillary Clinton had retained the support of these voters, she would have easily won the 2016 election, just as Obama had done four years earlier. Instead, 77 percent of Obama voters supported Clinton. Our survey data show that 10 percent of the former Obama voters cast ballots for Trump in 2016; 4 percent switched to minor parties; and 8 percent did not vote. Thus, our key research question is, what accounts for these defections? A second reason why we have chosen this focus is that restricting our analysis to former Obama supporters provides a form of control for other potentially confounding factors. It could not be argued, for example, that those who abandoned the Democratic candidate in 2016 were hostile to Democratic candidates, per se, or were implacable conservatives. 1 Philip N. Howard, Gillian Bolsover, Bence Kollanyi, Samantha Bradshaw and Lisa-Maria Neudert, Junk News and Bots During the U.S. Election: What Were Michigan Voters Sharing over Twitter?, COMPROP Data Memo, 2017.1, March 26, 2017.

Findings from Our Post-Election Survey Our post-election survey asked our respondents 281 questions that included, in addition to the standard election-survey items, three fake news statements. Two of these were negative statements about Hillary Clinton and one was a positive statement involving Donald Trump. All three were widely disseminated through social media and were picked up by the broadcast media as well. The first is the claim that Hillary Clinton is in very poor health due to a serious illness. 25 percent of all respondents in our nationally representative sample believed that this was definitely true or probably true, as did 12 percent of our former Obama supporters. The second is a statement that Pope Francis endorsed Donald Trump for president prior to the election. About 10 percent of our national sample and 8 percent of Obama supporters thought this statement was true. Finally, we asked our respondents if they believed that During her time as U.S. Secretary of State, Hillary Clinton approved weapon sales to Islamic jihadists, including ISIS. 35 percent of our national sample believed that Clinton had sold weapons to ISIS, as did 20 percent of former Obama voters. The statistical association between belief in these fake news stories and vote choice in the 2016 election by former Barack Obama supporters is very strong. Among those who believed none of the three fake news stories, 89 percent cast ballots for Hillary Clinton in 2016; among those who believed one fake news item, this level of electoral support fell to 61 percent; but among those who had voted for Obama in 2012 and believed two or all three of these false assertions, only 17 percent voted for Clinton (Tau-b correlation=.50). To be sure, causality cannot be proven on the basis of a single-wave survey like ours. While we shall interpret these numbers as evidence that belief in fake news stories led former Obama voters to abandon the Democratic candidate for president in 2016, it is also possible that the direction of causality is the reverse that someone who, for whatever reason, chose not to vote for Clinton might endorse these false statements (even if they had not heard them before) in order to rationalize his/her voting decision. We shall therefore explore a number of rival hypotheses to try to assess the possibility that abandonment of the Democratic presidential ticket might have been motivated by other factors, and then include all of these variables in a multivariate equation in which it is possible to control for these alternatives. Alternative Explanatory Factors The Clinton campaign heavily emphasized gender-related issues in an attempt to mobilize female voters. Could this have alienated men to the extent that they abandoned their 2012 support for the Democratic presidential candidate? Our data provide no support for such a claim: an identical 23 percent of both male and female respondents defected from the Democratic ticket. Did the disappearance of this country s first African-American president from the top of the Democratic ticket lead black voters to waiver in their commitment to the Democratic candidate? No. Indeed, fewer African-American voters (20 percent) defected from Clinton than did white voters (23 percent).

Age is weakly related to defection from the Democratic ticket in 2016: while 20 percent of voters over age 35 abandoned the Democratic ticket in 2016, 30 percent of younger voters did so (Tau-b =.10). Education is also weakly associated with defection: among college-educated former Obama voters, just 16 percent did not vote for Clinton, while 27 percent of those with lower educational attainment defected (Tau-b =.12). More overtly political variables had a stronger relationship with defection. 14 percent of those who placed themselves in the three most progressive or left positions on a 10-point ideological scale did not vote for Clinton, as compared with 50 percent of those at the conservative end of this continuum (Tau-b =.22). Similarly, dissatisfaction with the condition of the economy also contributed to defection from the Democratic camp: just 12 percent of those who thought that the current economic situation was good or very good abandoned Hillary Clinton, while 39 percent who regarded the economy as poor or very poor defected from the Democratic ticket (Tau-b =.24). Party identification had a more significant impact. Among those 2012 Obama voters who identified themselves as Democrats, 7 percent did not vote for Clinton; this rose to 40 percent among independents and to 68 percent among those who identified with the Republican, Libertarian or Green parties (Tau-b =.47), as the more heterogeneous Obama coalition shrank down to a smaller Democratic core of support for Clinton. The penultimate step in this analysis was to include all of these variables in two multivariate regression equations, one including the aforementioned seven alternative explanatory variables, and then a second to which the fake news index is added. The first equation including gender, race, age, education, ideological orientation, dissatisfaction with the condition of the economy, and party identification explains 38 percent (as measured by the Nagelkerke R Square statistic) of the defection of former Obama voters from the Democratic ticket in 2016. When the index of fake news items is added to the equation, the percentage of variance explained is increased by an additional 14 percent after the influence of all of the other variables has been taken into consideration. An even more compelling test of the independent impact of belief in fake news is to add to this multivariate analysis measures of the extent to which the respondent liked or disliked Hillary Clinton and Donald Trump. Not surprisingly, these variables are strongly associated with defection from the Democratic ticket in 2016 (Tau-b.50 and.49, respectively). If the association between defecting from Clinton and belief in fake news exposure were purely epiphenomenal i.e., that dislike for Clinton and/or positive affect toward Trump caused both belief in fake news and electoral defection then the introduction of these thermometer evaluations into the equation would eliminate the association between fake news and defection. Instead, while the independent impact of fake news is reduced in its explanatory power by the inclusion of the Clinton and Trump favorability scores, the fake news scale retains a significant impact, explaining 4 percent of the defections from Hillary Clinton. Using a different statistical measure (the odds ratio), former Obama voters who believed one or more of these fake news stories were 3.3 times more likely to defect from the Democratic ticket in 2016 than those who did not believe any of these false claims. We must reiterate that, given the inability to determine temporal order in a single-wave cross-sectional survey, we cannot prove that belief in fake news caused these former Obama voters to defect from the Democratic candidate in 2016. But if these estimates are even remotely

accurate as measures of the impact of belief in fake news on defections from the Democratic candidate, it is highly likely that this pernicious pollution of our political discourse was sufficient to influence the outcome of what was a very close election. Clinton lost the Presidency by 77,744 votes (0.6 percent) cast in the key battleground states of Pennsylvania, Michigan and Wisconsin. The impact of fake news appears from our data to be enough to account for that total. Methodological Appendix The data for this article came from an Internet survey conducted for the authors by YouGov, a prominent Internet survey organization, from December 5, 2016 through January 6, 2017. Initial contacts were matched down to a final sample of 1600 respondents on the basis of gender, age, race, education, ideology, and political interest. The sampling frame was constructed by stratified sampling from the full 2010 American Community Survey (ACS) sample with selection within strata by weighted sampling with replacements (using the person weights on the public use file). Data on voter registration status and turnout were matched to this frame using the November 2010 Current Population Survey. Data on interest in politics and party identification were then matched to this frame from the 2007 Pew Religious Life Survey. The matched cases were weighted to the sampling frame using propensity scores. The matched cases and the frame were combined and a logistic regression was estimated for inclusion in the frame. The propensity score function included age, gender, race/ethnicity, years of education, political interest, ideology, and census region. The propensity scores were grouped into deciles of the estimated propensity score in the frame and post-stratified according to these deciles. Most of the data presented in this article are frequencies that are self-explanatory (e.g., the percentage of former Obama voters who voted for Hillary Clinton). The final paragraphs, however, present the results of stepwise multivariate regression analyses of these data in which the dependent variable is defection by former Obama voters to Donald Trump, to another party, or to abstention. The full model of the final step in this analysis is presented below:

Binary Logit Regression Analysis of the Impact of Fake News on Defection from Clinton 2 B (s.e.) Wald Odds Confidence Interval Ratio Lower Upper Female -.33 (.36).9 1.4.56 3.5 White.20 (.41).2.8.29 2.4 Over Age 35.01 (.19).0 1.0.62 1.6 College Educated -.06 (.40).0.9.34 2.6 Economy: Poor/Very.29 (.54).3 1.3.33 5.4 Economy: Average.39 (.51).6 1.5.39 5.6 Left-right: Right.55 (.72).6 1.7.27 11.1 Left-right; Center -.56 (.40) 1.9.6.20 1.6 Party ID: None 1.33 (.42)** 10.2 3.8 1.29 11.0 Party ID: Republican 1.72 (.63)** 7.5 5.6 1.11 28.4 Trump: Neutral -1.18 (.71) 2.7.3.05 1.9 Trump: Unfavorable -2.24 (.62)*** 12.9.1.02.5 Clinton: Neutral.85 (.44) 3.7 2.3.75 7.2 Clinton: Unfavorable 1.99 (.47)*** 17.8 7.3 2.17 24.7 Believes Fake News 1.18 (.26)*** 20.0 3.3 1.65 6.4 Nagelkerke R 2 =.626 * Sig.@.05 ** Sig @ 0.01 *** Sig. @.000 2 In order to satisfy the assumptions underlying the Logit analysis, all variables were dichotomized, including of course the key dependent variable of voting for Clinton vs. defecting among the Obama voters. For predictor variables in the model that had more than two categories, dummy variables were created for each category of substantive interest vs. all other categories, leaving one category out of the model to serve as the baseline. For example, the dummy variables for Party ID are independents vs. all others and Republicans versus all others, with Democrats as the base category.the confidence interval was set at 99%.