THE VALUE OF JUDICIAL TRAINING IN QUANTITATIVE AND SCIENTIFIC METHODS

Similar documents
Preparing for Daubert Through the Life of a Case

Kumho Tire Co., Ltd. v. Carmichael. Case Background

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE WESTERN DISTRICT OF OKLAHOMA

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT EASTERN DISTRICT OF WISCONSIN. v. Case No. 16-CV-1396 DECISION AND ORDER

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE DISTRICT OF COLUMBIA

Evidentiary Standards in the State of Illinois: The Interpretation and Implementation of Supreme Court Opinions

THE NATIONAL CENTER FOR JUSTICE AND

Changes to Rule 702(a): Has North Carolina Codified Daubert and Does It Matter? During the past legislative session, the General Assembly changed Rule

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT WESTERN DISTRICT OF LOUISIANA LAKE CHARLES DIVISION * * * * * * * * *

MEMORANDUM OPINION & ORDER

United States District Court EASTERN DISTRICT OF TEXAS SHERMAN DIVISION

Misinterpretation and Misapplication of Kumho Tire to Business Valuation

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE WESTERN DISTRICT OF PENNSYLVANIA

Daubert Issues For Footwear Examiners

Court Appointed Scientific Experts A Handbook for Experts

Lighting Up the Post- Daubert Landscape?

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT EASTERN DISTRICT OF TEXAS TYLER DIVISION ORDER

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT EASTERN DISTRICT OF MICHIGAN SOUTHERN DIVISION. Defendant.

Case 1:15-cv MEH Document 58 Filed 05/10/16 USDC Colorado Page 1 of 11 IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE DISTRICT OF COLORADO

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE NORTHERN DISTRICT OF OHIO WESTERN DIVISION. CITY OF FINDLAY, et al.l, Defendant.

Case 2:11-cv RBS -DEM Document 94 Filed 10/31/12 Page 1 of 6 PageID# 2118

Case 2:14-cv SSV-JCW Document 130 Filed 06/09/15 Page 1 of 14 UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT EASTERN DISTRICT OF LOUISIANA VERSUS NO:

United States District Court EASTERN DISTRICT OF TEXAS SHERMAN DIVISION

Wal-Mart v. Dukes What s Next for Employment Class/Collective Actions

The Law Commission. The consultation. Dr Chris Pamplin 5/5/2009. The Expert Witness 1

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE DISTRICT OF COLORADO Judge Robert E. Blackburn

IN THE STATE COURT OF DEKALB COUNTY STATE OF GEORGIA

The Royalty Owners file this Response to Gertrude Petroleum Corporation s ( GPC )

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE SOUTHERN DISTRICT OF ILLINOIS MEMORANDUM AND ORDER

Overview of Admissibility of Expert Testimony

present photographic identification before casting ballots. Presently before the Court is

The Role of Experts in Class Certification in U.S. Antitrust Cases. Stacey Anne Mahoney Bingham McCutchen LLP

IN THE CIRCUIT COURT OF WOOD COUNTY, WEST VIRGINIA. // Case No. 02-F-131 (Thomas C Evans, III, Judge)

THE ROLE OF THE EXPERT IN MARITIME MATTERS - AN OUTLINE OF LEGAL AND PRACTICAL CONSIDERATIONS

IN THE SUPERIOR COURT OF THE STATE OF DELAWARE IN AND FOR NEW CASTLE COUNTY. STATE OF DELAWARE ) ) v. ) ID No: ) BRADFORD JONES )

Qualifying a Witness as an Expert Using the Daubert Standard

CHRISTIAN V. GRAY: THE OKLAHOMA SUPREME COURT ACCEPTS THE DAUBERT STANDARD

Class Certification in Complex Commercial Litigation

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT EASTERN DISTRICT OF MICHIGAN SOUTHERN DIVISION UNITED STATES OF AMERICA, CRIMINAL NO

28a USC 702. NB: This unofficial compilation of the U.S. Code is current as of Jan. 5, 2009 (see

Comcast Corp. et al. v. Behrend et al. Docket No Argument Date: November 5, 2012 From: The Third Circuit

Rumberger KIRK & CALDWELL

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT EASTERN DISTRICT OF MISSOURI EASTERN DIVISION

Case 1:08-cr CCB Document 64 Filed 12/08/09 Page 1 of 6 IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE DISTRICT OF MARYLAND

THE UNFINISHED DAUBERT REVOLUTION

JUNK SCIENCE OR. EXPERT TESTIMONY? Clinical Professor Kate Mewhinney

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE NORTHERN DISTRICT OF TEXAS DALLAS DIVISION

Judicial Hellholes: Don t Get Burned Risk Management Techniques and Defense Strategies for Litigating in Plaintiff Friendly Jurisdictions

Neil Feldscher, CIH, CSP, Esq. and Chip Darius, MA, OHST

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT EASTERN DISTRICT OF LOUISIANA VERSUS NO ORDER AND REASONS

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT EASTERN DISTRICT OF TEXAS MARSHALL DIVISION. CORE WIRELESS LICENSING S.A.R.L., Case No. 2:14-cv-911-JRG-RSP (lead) v.

IN THE UNITED STATES COURT OF APPEALS FOR THE FIFTH CIRCUIT

IN THE DISTRICT COURT OF GEARY COUNTY, KANSAS BACKGROUND

SERVING AS A RETAINED EXPERT WITNESS THE SUCCESSFUL EXPERT EXPERIENCE: PRACTICAL TIPS FOR SERVING WELL AND GETTING PAID

Court granted Defendants motion in limine to preclude the testimony of Plaintiffs damages

Daubert Case Summaries

BATTLE OF THE EXPERTS: HOW TO EFFECTIVELY MANAGE AND LEVERAGE EXPERTS FOR OPTIMAL RESULTS

IN THE SUPREME COURT OF THE STATE OF IDAHO Docket No ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) )

RESPONSEt EVALUATING MERGER ENFORCEMENT DURING THE OBAMA ADMINISTRATION

[J ] IN THE SUPREME COURT OF PENNSYLVANIA EASTERN DISTRICT : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : :

Daubert and Rule 702: Effectively Presenting and Challenging Experts in Federal Court

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE DISTRICT OF COLORADO Judge Raymond P. Moore

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT SOUTHERN DISTRICT OF INDIANA INDIANAPOLIS DIVISION

Reverse Payment Settlements In Pharma Industry: Revisited

Book Review: Carl Cranor, Toxic Torts: Science, Law, and the Possibility of Justice

Before HATCHETT, Chief Judge, HULL, Circuit Judge, and MOORE *, District Judge.

JUNK SCIENCE OR. EXPERT TESTIMONY? Clinical Professor Kate Mewhinney

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE EASTERN DISTRICT OF TEXAS MARSHALL DIVISION DAUBERT ORDER

Written materials by Jonathan D. Sasser

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE EASTERN DISTRICT OF TEXAS MARSHALL DIVISION

MOTION TO EXCLUDE UNRELIABLE EVIDENCE/MOTION IN LIMINE (CHLOROFORM)

BEGELMAN & ORLOW, P.C. Attorneys at Law

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE SOUTHERN DISTRICT OF TEXAS HOUSTON DIVISION

Learning from Small Subsamples without Cherry Picking: The Case of Non-Citizen Registration and Voting

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT SOUTHERN DISTRICT OF CALIFORNIA

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT MIDDLE DISTRICT OF FLORIDA FORT MYERS DIVISION. v. Case No: 2:17-cv-656-FtM-29UAM OPINION AND ORDER

Ninth Coordination Meeting on International Migration

Segal and Howard also constructed a social liberalism score (see Segal & Howard 1999).

Reliable Analysis Is Key To Addressing Ascertainability

Case 1:06-cv Document 695 Filed 02/23/10 Page 1 of 7 IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE NORTHERN DISTRICT OF ILLINOIS EASTERN DIVISION

Case: 2:16-cv CDP Doc. #: 162 Filed: 12/03/18 Page: 1 of 5 PageID #: 8273

UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT SOUTHERN DISTRICT OF FLORIDA FORT LAUDERDALE DIVISION CASE NO CR-FERGUSON REPORT AND RECOMMENDATION

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE DISTRICT OF KANSAS. MDL No SCHEDULING ORDER NO. 2

IN THE COURT OF CRIMINAL APPEALS OF TEXAS

scc Doc 860 Filed 03/06/12 Entered 03/06/12 16:37:03 Main Document Pg 1 of 14

COUNTY. STATE OF NORTH CAROLINA ) ) MOTION TO EXCLUDE vs. ) TESTIMONY REGARDING ) FIELD SOBRIETY TESTS, ) Defendant. ) I.

Opinion Evidence. Penny J. White May 2015

Case Study: Get out the Vote

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE WESTERN DISTRICT OF PENNSYLVANIA ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) MEMORANDUM OPINION

RULES OF EVIDENCE LEGAL STANDARDS

EXPERT WITNESS: A COMPUTER SCIENCE EMPHASIS

Give a brief description of case, particularly the. confession at issue and the pertinent circumstances surrounding

2:12-cr SFC-MKM Doc # 227 Filed 12/06/13 Pg 1 of 12 Pg ID 1213 UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT EASTERN DISTRICT OF MICHIGAN SOUTHERN DIVISION

Terry and Substantive Law

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT WESTERN DISTRICT OF OKLAHOMA

INTEL AND THE DEATH OF U.S. ANTITRUST LAW

THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE WESTERN DISTRICT OF MISSOURI CENTRAL DIVISION

Journals in the Discipline: A Report on a New Survey of American Political Scientists

IN THE UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT FOR THE EASTERN DISTRICT OF TEXAS MARSHALL DIVISION MEMORANDUM OPINION AND ORDER

STATE OF MICHIGAN COURT OF APPEALS

Transcription:

DRAFT, COMMENTS WELCOME THE VALUE OF JUDICIAL TRAINING IN QUANTITATIVE AND SCIENTIFIC METHODS LEC White Paper, April 8, 2013

Introduction Judges face a significant skills gap that threatens to undermine their ability to properly adjudicate civil matters. Although modern disputes frequently involve decisions that hinge on quantitative or scientific evidence, few judges have a background sufficient to prepare them for these decisions. Courses on statistics, economics, and finance are almost entirely absent in legal education programs even though judicial analysis of these matters affects untold resources throughout the world on a daily basis. With few exceptions, the individuals who sort into law do not have scientific or quantitative backgrounds. In the US, the majority of law school applicants have undergraduate degrees in the humanities and the non-quantitative social sciences. 1 In most of the rest of the world, where law is a first degree, the situation is likely even worse. Judges are not unaware of this problem. Tasked by the Supreme Court with serving as the gatekeeper with respect to scientific evidence, the 9 th Circuit opinion in the follow up to Daubert laments: As we read the Supreme Court s teaching in Daubert, therefore, though we are largely untrained in science and certainly no match for any of the witnesses whose testimony we are reviewing, it is our responsibility to determine whether those experts proposed testimony amounts to scientific knowledge, constitutes good science, and was derived by the scientific method. 2 In a presentation at the 2008 symposium The Supreme Court and Useful Knowledge, Linda Greenhouse, the Pulitzer Prize winning reporter who covered the Supreme Court for the New York Times from 1978 to 2007, concluded A good judge is not necessarily one who knows everything, but one who is willing to learn. 3 Presumably judges do want to get these issues right, and while most are likely willing to learn, 4 at least in theory, reality often gets in the way. Given the demands placed upon judges in most jurisdictions, it is unlikely they will have resources or the inclination to remedy this shortcoming despite recognizing the problem, especially in an environment where the remuneration of judges appears to fall short of compensating them for doing even the bare minimum. As Chief Justice John Roberts notes repeatedly, poor judicial pay is already depleting the ranks of the federal judiciary. 5 Under such circumstances, it is unreasonable to think judges will be motivated to incur the expense of improving their facility with scientific evidence on their own. Also, given that the gap between judicial pay and private practice pay is largest in the area of corporate law, it is likely that those with some background knowledge of finance and microeconomics will be especially under-represented within the judiciary. As recognized by the Court in Daubert, however, it is not reasonable to simply hope that the adversarial posture of the US courts will lead to a scenario where sound science (or statistics, or economics, etc) trumps bad science. Junk science can 1 See Nieswiadomy, Michael, LSAT Scores of Economics Majors: The 2008-2009 Class Update (June 25, 2009). Available at SSRN: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1430654 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1430654. 2 Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharms., Inc., 43 F.3d 1311, 1316 (9th Cir. 1995). 3 Greenhouse, Linda (2010), How Do Judges Know What They Know? Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 154(3): 287-293. 4 However, many judges may view this as a practical impossibility or even beyond the scope of their duties. David L. Faigman quotes a remarkable line by Justice Brennan from Craig v. Boren (429 U.S. 190 (1976)) while rejecting statistical evidence: There is no reason to belabor this line of analysis. It is unrealistic to expect either members of the judiciary or state officials to be well versed in the rigors of experimental or statistical technique. But this merely illustrates that proving broad sociological propositions by statistics is a dubious business... Faigman expresses shock at this statement, suggesting that it would be unthinkable that a Supreme Court justice would be as willing to disclaim any ability to read history, or presumably a similar unwillingness to wrestle with arguments grounded in tools from the liberal arts. See David L. Faigman, Judges As Amateur Scientists, 86 Boston University Law Review 1207, 1210-1211 (2006). 5 For the first public instance of this, see John Roberts, 2005 Year-End Report on The Federal Judiciary 3 (2006). LEC White Paper, Draft of April 8, 2013 Page 1 of 7

always be dressed up in elegant-sounding claims, wherein the more charismatic expert with the more aesthetically pleasing flow charts beats the bumbling but methodologically sound expert. Inquisitorial systems generate their own problems with respect to scientific or quantitative evidence as well. While such systems may be less susceptible to manipulation due to hired-gun experts in the courtroom, the problem does not go away as the parties in front of the judge will have used such experts in framing the dispute and preparing their arguments. Further, even if the judge in such a system can rely on a hopefully disinterested court-appointed expert for guidance, lacking any background skills in these areas, the judge will be poorly positioned to choose an adequately trained expert. Beyond that, often the core of a dispute involves judging how relevant a scientific finding or literature is in answering a particular legal question. Non-legal experts are often ill equipped for such inquiries that require some familiarity with both the science and the law. Thus, reliance on a judge s own ambition to be a better consumer of scientific and quantitative evidence is overly optimistic; nor will institutional safeguards remedy the knowledge deficit of judges worldwide. While this challenge may seem insurmountable, the potential to educate judges has been demonstrated in the U.S. setting. This report highlights some of the success stories for judicial education. The first involves the publication of the Federal Judicial Center s Manual on Scientific Evidence in the wake of the Supreme Court s Daubert ruling which left federal judges in the clear role of gatekeepers regarding expert evidence. Qualitative evidence suggests that judges are indeed interested in easily available, well-done resources to improve their ability to make judgments regarding scientific and quantitative evidence. There are indications that the influence of the FJC s work spread beyond the nominal audience of the federal judiciary, potentially improving the work of state judges as well. The second example involves a recent decision by the DC Circuit that involved analyzing the reliability and relevance of quantitative research about the efficacy of graphic health warnings on cigarettes. The literature on the effects of these warnings exhibits substantial problems with respect to the relevance inquiry, as well as some potential reliability problems, though these problems were never highlighted within the literature itself, nor were they acknowledged by the U.S. regulator that was a party to the case. For all of the reasons suggested above, this could have been a situation where a lack of scientific and quantitative understanding on the judges part allowed these shortcomings to go unnoticed. However, two of the three judges on the panel had received some privately-funded training in issues of research design and statistical methods, and, perhaps not unrelatedly, these judges found that the scientific basis for graphic health warnings fell short of the relevant legal threshold. The dissenting judge had not received such training. While it is not possible to isolate the training as the motivation for the DC Circuit case on graphic health warnings separately from other plausible explanations, it is certainly a persuasive case study. However, drawing strong inferences from a single case study is unwarranted. There is, however, more systematic evidence on the effects of this kind of judicial training in the area of antitrust. The evidence is compelling there that judges who have received post-law school training, generally privately funded, in the areas of microeconomics, finance, and statistics make systematically better decisions, and the result cannot be explained by random chance or through the other mechanisms that may explain the behavior of the judges in the graphic health warnings case. As the following discussions demonstrate, providing judges with attractive and low-cost educational opportunities and resources has the potential to mitigate the gaps they otherwise have in their backgrounds. Expanding such opportunities both in the US and abroad will likely generate a very high social return. Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence Beginning in the early 1990s, there was a general recognition that there was a need to educate federal judges in matters related to scientific evidence. One manifestation of this was the creation of the Federal Judicial Center s Science and LEC White Paper, Draft of April 8, 2013 Page 2 of 7

Technology Resources Center (STRC). In addition to developing educational programs, the STRC was tasked with developing a handbook on science and technology for judges. 6 The perceived need for this kind of guidance was heightened when the Supreme Court abandoned the traditional Frye rule in Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc. 7 Whereas the Frye rule had limited the inquiry regarding the admissibility of scientific evidence to asking whether the evidence was generally accepted in the relevant academic community, Daubert demanded that judges themselves serve a gatekeeper function, evaluating the soundness of the underlying methodology used by the expert to arrive at her conclusions. This inquiry involved a determination of whether the underlying methods were reliable and whether they were appropriately applied in the given legal context (i.e., is the application relevant to the legal point being advanced by the evidence). While the Court did provide some potential indicators of methodological soundness (i.e., whether the expert s theory could be tested/falsified; whether the theory had been subjected to a peer review process; whether the error rate of an expert s empirical method was known; acceptance in the relevant scientific community), the discretion of the judge in making these determinations was left quite broad. This discretion point was later made clear in General Electric Co. v. Joiner which held that the appellate standard on an admissibility determination under Daubert is abuse of discretion. 8 The final case in the Daubert trilogy, Kumho Tire Co. v. Carmichael, made clear that this standard applied even to fields like economics and finance, in addition to the hard sciences. 9 The Federal Judicial Center published the first Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence in 1994, with subsequent editions published in 2000 and 2011. The manual, written with the aim of being accessible to individuals with no scientific or quantitative training, includes chapters on statistics, multiple regression, survey research, microeconomics, epidemiology, and toxicology, among other technical fields. The judiciary embraced this manual. In preparation for the third edition, the Federal Judicial Center engaged in survey research which found that judges at both the state and federal levels consult the manual when they are faced with scientific or technical evidence. This reinforces the FJC s view that the manual has been highly successful based on the fact that it has sold more than 100,000 copies. Even this latter evidence is likely understated, given that the manual is available for free in an electronic format. 10 In its assessment of the impact of the manual, the Federal Judicial Center concludes: The Manual continues to be an extremely important resource for judges handling cases in which scientific evidence plays a role. While no manual for general distribution can anticipate and address the particular questions a judge or jury may need to decide, the Manual appears to provide a general introduction that can help judges dealing with scientific issues pertaining to the subject of a dispute. The Manual has gained a reputation for providing judges with a frame of reference to approach such disputes with confidence and with a sufficient level of comfort to listen, learn, and ultimately make a decision on a matter involving scientific content. 11 This conclusion accords with some empirical evidence on the matter suggesting that judges have become much more comfortable in questioning expert evidence during the period where this manual has been available. A 2001 RAND study found that post 1994, federal judges were more likely to challenge expert evidence and to ultimately exclude said evidence. 12 6 For background on these developments, see Carnegie Commission on Science, Technology, and Government (1993), Science and Technology in Judicial Decision Making: Creating Opportunities and Meeting Challenges. 7 509 U.S. 579 (1993). 8 522 U.S. 136 (1997). 9 526 U.S. 137 (1999). 10 For more information on this claim, see Committee on the Evaluation of the Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence; National Research Council, Evaluation of the Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence: Letter Report (2009). 11 Id, 16. 12 See Lloyd Dixon and Brian Gill (2001), Changes in the Standards for Admitting Expert Evidence in Federal Civil Cases Since the Daubert Decision, RAND Monograph Report. LEC White Paper, Draft of April 8, 2013 Page 3 of 7

While RAND speculated that this was a function of the more demanding Daubert rule, this conclusion is found to be lacking in work comparing federal and state courts in this period. A 2005 Virginia Law Review article by Edward Cheng and Albert Yoon, found no meaningful difference between state jurisdictions retaining the old Frye rule and state courts adopting the Daubert rule as well as federal courts. Their study focused on analyzing changes in removal rates to federal court after Daubert is adopted. The intuition behind this research design is that if judges are more demanding under Daubert regarding scientific evidence, defendants will have a stronger incentive to remove the case to federal court when a plaintiff files in a state court operating under Frye than if the plaintiff files in a state court that has already adopted Daubert. They find no evidence of a differential effect, stating, [Daubert] has a vanishingly small effect on removal rate. DAUBERT contributes only five-thousandths of a percentage point to a state's removal rate, and the result is statistically insignificant. This result suggests that, in making removal decisions, defendants place little weight on whether a state follows Frye or Daubert. 13 For Daubert to not make a difference between state and federal court practice, as found in Cheng and Yoon, but to have been associated with increasing scrutiny at the federal level, as found in the RAND study, it is likely the case that state courts were becoming increasingly demanding as well. While we cannot be certain judges were more demanding due to their reliance on the Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence, the survey results collected from judges by the Federal Judicial Center surely indicate that the judges themselves claim the manual has had an effect. However, as the RAND study makes clear, this increasing scrutiny may not necessarily imply that judges are handling scientific evidence better. Perhaps during this period, judges were excluding evidence that is scientifically valid. While a systematic study of this possibility is likely impossible, since it would require making independent determinations regarding an unmanageable number of evidence determinations by scholars with expertise in both law and science, there is some indirect evidence that the state of science in the courtroom was actually improving in this time period. In data collected by Eric Helland and Jonathan Klick for their 2012 article Does Anyone Get Stopped at the Gate? An Empirical Assessment of the Daubert Trilogy in the States, 14 they too confirm the Cheng and Yoon result that Daubert itself does not make much of a difference on objective indicators of an expert s quality (publication record, years of experience, affiliation with a top 10 university) but they do find that these indicators are improving everywhere, Daubert adopters and non-adopters alike, over time, suggesting that the increasing demands identified by RAND are likely the result of an improvement in the rigor of the evidence that gets admitted. Again, this improvement in the demands made by judges in the post-daubert era shows up in both state and federal courts. While there are likely many factors driving this, judges themselves appear to credit the Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence. However, the FJC s survey results suggest that the judges believe they could substantially benefit from actual training in the topics covered in the manual. Specifically, the FJC commented, Judges expressed interest in educational programs that would allow them to work through material encountered at Daubert hearings. A Science for Judges Program on Evidence-Based Medicine attempted to accommodate this desire by placing participants in small break-out groups to analyze studies from speaker presentations. Judges liked this format. The advantages of this model might be harnessed using interactive computer exercises dealing with, for example, statistical issues. 15 While the FJC has organized some limited training sessions, this demand goes largely unmet via public channels. There are, however, more extensive privately-financed programs that apparently generate substantial improvements to judicial human capital. 13 Edward K. Cheng and Albert H. Yoon, Does Frye or Daubert Matter? A Study of Scientific Admissibility Standards, 91 Virginia Law Review 471, 498 (2005). 14 Eric Helland and Jonathan Klick, Does Anyone Get Stopped at the Gate? An Empirical Assessment of the Daubert Trilogy in the States, 20 Supreme Court Economic Review (2012). 15 Committee on the Evaluation of the Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence; National Research Council, Evaluation of the Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence: Letter Report (2009), 12. LEC White Paper, Draft of April 8, 2013 Page 4 of 7

R.J. Reynolds Tobacco Company v. Food and Drug Administration In the 2012 appellate decision, Circuit Judge Janice Rogers Brown and Senior Circuit Judge Raymond Randolph upheld a lower court ruling that stopped the imposition of graphic health warnings on cigarette packs, while Circuit Judge Judith Rogers dissented. It is interesting to note that Judge Randolph has been a frequent participant in privately financed judicial education programs covering statistical methods, and Judge Rogers Brown was an attendee of such programs while a state judge in California. Judge Rogers, however, has not participated in programs of this nature. While much of the DC Circuit opinion focuses on the standard of review for compelled commercial speech, largely agreeing with district court Judge Richard Leon s opinion that the graphic health warnings did not represent the kind of narrowly tailored regulation required in compelled speech nor does the government have a compelling interest in simply advocating that the public not purchase a legal product 16, its opinion also demonstrated a high degree of sophistication about both the reliability and the relevance of the government s statistical evidence. The dissenting opinion by Rogers does not engage the statistical evidence, merely accepting the FDA s implicit claim that it is both reliable and relevant. Putting aside the significant reliability and, more importantly, relevance problems that plague the graphic health warnings literature generally, the FDA s own evidence that undergirds the agency s final rule is unsound from both a reliability and relevance perspective. The DC Circuit opinion makes this point clearly. Zeroing in on the relevance problems, the DC Circuit opinion states: FDA has not provided a shred of evidence much less the substantial evidence required by the APA showing that the graphic warnings will directly advance its interest in reducing the number of Americans who smoke. FDA makes much of the international consensus surrounding the effectiveness of large graphic warnings, but offers no evidence showing that such warnings have directly caused a material decrease in smoking rates in any of the countries that now require them. While studies of Canadian and Australian youth smokers showed that the warnings on cigarette packs caused a substantial number of survey participants to think or think more about quitting smoking, Proposed Rule at 69,532, and FDA might be correct that intentions are a necessary precursor to behavior change, Final Rule at 36,642, it is mere speculation to suggest that respondents who report increased thoughts about quitting smoking will actually follow through on their intentions. And at no point did these studies attempt to evaluate whether the increased thoughts about smoking cessation led participants to actually quit. Another Australian study reported increased quit attempts by survey participants after that country enacted large graphic warnings, but found no association with short-term quit success. Proposed Rule at 9,532. Some Canadian and Australian studies indicated that large graphic warnings might induce individual smokers to reduce consumption, or to help persons who have already quit smoking remain abstinent. See id. But again, the study did not purport to show that the implementation of large graphic warnings has actually led to a reduction in smoking rates. 17 Rather than simply relying on the purported consensus, the DC Circuit majority properly examined whether the proffered evidence was actually relevant to the legal question, namely whether the graphic health warnings reduce actual smoking rates. Because all of the existing evidence involved subjective impressions or intentions, it is not relevant to a determination of what is likely to happen to the smoking incidence, the metric that relates to the government s interest, as opposed to intentions, which are not the object of government interest. While it is possible that intentions in this area are predictive of eventual behavior, there are no published peer reviewed studies demonstrating this link. 16 United States District Court for the District of Columbia, RJ Reynolds Tobacco Company et al v. United States Food and Drug Administration Civil Case No. 11-1482 (RJL) Memorandum Opinion (February 29, 2012), p. 16. 17 United States Court of Appeal for the District of Columbia Circuit, R.J. Reynolds Tobacco Company, et al v Food and Drug Administration et al (December 24, 2012), p. 25. LEC White Paper, Draft of April 8, 2013 Page 5 of 7

The majority also demonstrates its understanding of the simultaneity, or omitted variables bias, problem that plagues some of the other evidence the FDA relies on. In the year prior to the introduction of graphic warnings, the Canadian national survey showed that 24 percent of Canadians aged 15 or older smoked cigarettes. In 2001, the year the warnings were introduced, the national smoking rate dropped to 22 percent, and it further dropped to 21 percent in 2002. Id. at 69,532. But the raw numbers don t tell the whole tale. FDA concedes it cannot directly attribute any decrease in the Canadian smoking rate to the graphic warnings because the Canadian government implemented other smoking control initiatives, including an increase in the cigarette tax and new restrictions on public smoking, during the same period. 18 That is, while the FDA suggested that the observed decline in smoking rates in Canada post-2000 was due to that country s introduction of graphic warnings, the court recognizes that Canada introduced a whole menu of smoking regulations, most importantly additional cigarette taxes, to say nothing of the fact that smoking had been trending downward in Canada for quite some time prior to the introduction of graphic health warnings. The DC Circuit opinion shows a court that is both comfortable and sophisticated in analyzing the application of quantitative evidence to a legal dispute, while the dissent is much more passive in its reliance on the government s presentation of the evidence. The dissent does little to answer the majority s incisive critique of the agency s evidence. While this may be a reflection of Judge Rogers s legal position, it could also indicate a lack of comfort and sophistication with the evidence on her part. As for the source of Judge Rogers Brown and Randolph s facility with the quantitative evidence, it is not possible to know for sure. However, it is not unreasonable to infer that the training each received through programs covering statistical methods and research design, such as those offered by the Law and Economics Center, may have had a positive effect. The Effect of Training in Antitrust Law Although the DC Circuit experience with graphic health warnings is interesting and hints at the potentially positive effects of privately financed judicial training programs, at the end of the day, it is not possible to isolate that training as the source of the majority s statistical sophistication. Further, even if it were possible to use that case study as evidence of the value of such training, it is a single case that may not generalize. There is, however, a more systematic treatment of the effects of this kind of training on the sophistication of judges in the area of antitrust law. In a 2011 article in the Journal of Law and Economics, Michael Baye and Joshua Wright analyze data on the effect of attending judicial training seminars organized by the Law and Economics Center of George Mason University and performance in antitrust cases. 19 Their study analyzed 714 antitrust cases over the period 1996-2006, suggesting a fairly broad sample unlikely to be inordinately affected by a few idiosyncratic judges or cases. Antitrust is an area where judges are especially likely to be under-prepared. Modern antitrust law has departed substantially from its historic approach of applying per se rules to challenged activities. Under modern law, most cases are decided via a rule of reason approach that examines the effect of the challenged activity on consumer welfare as judged by modern economic theory and sophisticated statistical analyses. Presumably because of this, an ABA survey found that less than one fourth of antitrust economists responded that judges usually understand the economic issues in an antitrust case. 20 For a measure of performance, Baye and Wright examine the appeals rate on the assumption that a higher quality decision at the trial stage is less likely to be appealed. They also code the decisions according to their economic complexity, focusing on whether the case involved complicated econometric evidence, game theory, or other elements requiring greater economic sophistication. 18 Id, p. 26. 19 Michael R. Baye and Joshua D. Wright (2011), Is Antitrust Too Complicated for Generalist Judges? The Impact of Economic Complexity and Judicial Training on Appeals, Journal of Law and Economics, 54(1): 1-24. 20 See Baker, Jonathan B., and M. Howard Morse. 2006. Final Report of the American Bar Association Antitrust Division Economic Evidence Task Force. Chicago: American Bar Association. http://www.abanet.org/antitrust/at-reports/01-c-ii.pdf. LEC White Paper, Draft of April 8, 2013 Page 6 of 7

In short, after controlling for trends over time, they find that cases decided by judges who have gone through training programs were about 10 percent less likely to be appealed, and this result is statistically significant. The basic result holds even when the authors control for the specific type of antitrust case (e.g., merger, monopolization, etc), the plaintiff type (private, DOJ, FTC, or state Attorney General), and the federal circuit where the case was decided. Interestingly, this effect was most pronounced in the more simple cases, suggesting that the value of the training is primarily in remedying fairly basic skills deficits. To reinforce confidence in the positive effects of judicial training, Baye and Wright replicate their results after including controls for the antitrust experience of the judge, the political party of the judge, and a measure of whether the judge has an advanced degree. These controls mitigate concerns that the results are driven by certain judges, who might happen to be naturally better positioned to write a high quality antitrust opinion, being more likely to attend programs at the Law and Economics Center. By including a measure of experience, Baye and Wright are also able to conclude that on the job training does not appear to be as effective as basic judicial training. While the effect of privately financed judicial training has not been studied systematically in other areas, these antitrust results suggest that an expansion of such opportunities could significantly improve judicial performance. A better equipped judiciary is likely to improve social welfare substantially. Conclusion Every day judges are asked to make extremely important decisions, potentially worth millions of dollars, which hinge on complicated determinations involving scientific and quantitative evidence. Judges are generally ill-equipped to make these determinations. Basic economics, statistics, and science are subjects that are absent in legal education throughout the world. Judges generally do not have the time, resources, or inclination to get themselves up to speed on these issues, much to the detriment of legal outcomes. Fortunately, there are some reasons to be optimistic. The popularity of the FJC s Reference Manual on Scientific Evidence shows that judges will avail themselves of cheaply available high quality resources. The performance of judges who receive privately funded training gives even more reason to be optimistic, as demonstrated clearly in the antitrust area. Other specific anecdotes, like the performance of the DC Circuit in its graphic health warnings case, reinforce this hope. Given the importance of a well-trained judiciary, in the context of under-funded judicial systems, there should be an attempt to expand these educational opportunities and to encourage judges to make use of them. LEC White Paper, Draft of April 8, 2013 Page 7 of 7