Intentional Undervotes in Presidential Elections, Tom W. Smith. NORCIUniversity of Chicago. December, GSS Topical Report No.

Similar documents
Unrecorded Votes and Political Representation. David C. Kimball. Chris T. Owens. and. Katherine McAndrew Keeney

IT MUST BE MANDATORY FOR VOTERS TO CHECK OPTICAL SCAN BALLOTS BEFORE THEY ARE OFFICIALLY CAST Norman Robbins, MD, PhD 1,

Who s Afraid of an Undervote? David C. Kimball University of Missouri-St. Louis Chris Owens Texas A&M University

What s Remaining to Do Versus What s Not: North Carolina Elections After the Help America Vote Act

Voided Ballot in the 1996 Presidential Election: A County-Level Analysis

U.S. Catholics split between intent to vote for Kerry and Bush.

Data Models. 1. Data REGISTRATION STATUS VOTING HISTORY

Partisan Nation: The Rise of Affective Partisan Polarization in the American Electorate

Misvotes, Undervotes, and Overvotes: the 2000 Presidential Election in Florida

Unsuccessful Provisional Voting in the 2008 General Election David C. Kimball and Edward B. Foley

Basic Election Admin Facts Need for Data By Kimball Brace, President Election Data Services, Inc. Denver June, 2014

BLISS INSTITUTE 2006 GENERAL ELECTION SURVEY

The Effect of North Carolina s New Electoral Reforms on Young People of Color

Study Background. Part I. Voter Experience with Ballots, Precincts, and Poll Workers

NATIONAL: 2018 HOUSE RACE STABILITY

THE WORKMEN S CIRCLE SURVEY OF AMERICAN JEWS. Jews, Economic Justice & the Vote in Steven M. Cohen and Samuel Abrams

Assessing Election Reform Four Years After Florida. David C. Kimball University of Missouri-St. Louis and

CIRCLE The Center for Information & Research on Civic Learning & Engagement. Youth Voting in the 2004 Battleground States

CIRCLE The Center for Information & Research on Civic Learning & Engagement 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10%

Secretary of State to postpone the October 7, 2003 recall election, on the ground that the use of

Changes in Party Identification among U.S. Adult Catholics in CARA Polls, % 48% 39% 41% 38% 30% 37% 31%

Iowa Voting Series, Paper 4: An Examination of Iowa Turnout Statistics Since 2000 by Party and Age Group

Central Florida Puerto Ricans Findings from 403 Telephone interviews conducted in June / July 2017.

Moral Values Take Back Seat to Partisanship and the Economy In 2004 Presidential Election

Union Voters and Democrats

Undervoting and Overvoting in the 2002 and 2006 Florida Gubernatorial Elections Are Touch Screens the Solution?

Res Publica 29. Literature Review

Chapter 9: Elections, Campaigns, and Voting. American Democracy Now, 4/e

The Cook Political Report / LSU Manship School Midterm Election Poll

Florida Atlantic University Poll: Trump Edging Clinton in Florida; Murphy and Rubio poised for tough Senate race

CIRCLE The Center for Information & Research on Civic Learning & Engagement

Asian American Survey

Number 2 Political Preferences of American Catholics at the Time of Election 2000

Global Warming and the 2008 Presidential Election

Chapter 7 Political Parties: Essential to Democracy

Ballot Design and Unrecorded Votes in the 2002 Midterm Election

Hispanics and the Changing Racial Demographics of the Intermountain West

I-4 Hispanics of Puerto Rican Origin Puerto Rico Statehood Council Dates: 8/20 9/4/ interviews / MoE +/- 4.9%

NEW JERSEY: TIGHT RACE IN CD03

D A T A D I C T I O N A R Y D2 D A T A D I C T I O N A R Y

CALTECH/MIT VOTING TECHNOLOGY PROJECT A

DUI Arrest Not a Factor, So Far SLIGHT BUSH MARGIN HOLDING WITH DAYS TO GO

State of the Facts 2018

American Cancer Society Cancer Action Network, November

Wide and growing divides in views of racial discrimination

Nevada Poll Results Tarkanian 39%, Heller 31% (31% undecided) 31% would renominate Heller (51% want someone else, 18% undecided)

Presidential Race Nip and Tuck in Michigan

Latinos and the Mid- term Election

St. Pete Pier Committee Members,

Colorado 2014: Comparisons of Predicted and Actual Turnout

Political Attitudes &Participation: Campaigns & Elections. State & Local Government POS 2112 Ch 5

Straight-Party Ballot Options and State Legislative Elections. David C. Kimball University of Missouri-St. Louis

Elections and Voting Behavior

POLI 300 Fall 2010 PROBLEM SET #5B: ANSWERS AND DISCUSSION

POLL DATA HIGHLIGHTS SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES BETWEEN REGISTERED DEMOCRATS AND REPUBLICANS.

It s Democrats +8 in Likely Voter Preference, With Trump and Health Care on Center Stage

Better Design Better Elections. A review of design flaws and solutions in recent national elections

HIGH POINT UNIVERSITY POLL MEMO RELEASE 9/24/2018 (UPDATE)

Michigan 14th Congressional District Democratic Primary Election Exclusive Polling Study for Fox 2 News Detroit.

Florida s District 13 Election in 2006: Can Statistics Tell Us Who Won?

Race, Immigration and America s s Changing Electorate. William H. Frey The Brookings Institution

THE 2004 YOUTH VOTE MEDIA COVERAGE. Select Newspaper Reports and Commentary

McLAUGHLIN & ASSOCIATES NATIONAL SURVEY OF REPUBLICANS TABLE OF CONTENTS FEBRUARY 28,

Building a Redistricting Database. By Kimball Brace Election Data Services, Inc.

The Youth Vote 2004 With a Historical Look at Youth Voting Patterns,

Chapter 6: Voters and Voter Behavior Section 4

Release #2475 Release Date: Wednesday, July 2, 2014 WHILE CALIFORNIANS ARE DISSATISFIED

NUMBERS, FACTS AND TRENDS SHAPING THE WORLD. FOR RELEASE September 12, 2014 FOR FURTHER INFORMATION ON THIS REPORT:

National Latino Leader? The Job is Open

CODEBOOK: American National Election Study Panel Subset (anespanl.sav)

Exposing Media Election Myths

Unit #2: Political Beliefs/Political Behaviors AP US Government & Politics Mr. Coia

GOP leads on economy, Democrats on health care, immigration

ONLINE SEGMENTS DATA DICTIONARY

Bush Inches Above 50%; First-Timers are a Wildcard

Young Voters in the 2010 Elections

9/1/11. Key Terms. Key Terms, cont.

Political Beliefs and Behaviors

Please note: additional data sources are referenced throughout this presentation, including national exit polls and NBC/WSJ national survey data.

Possible voting reforms in the United States

November 2018 Hidden Tribes: Midterms Report

Ballot Initiatives and Residual Ballots in the 2004 Presidential Election. David C. Kimball University of Missouri-St. Louis

AARP Pre-First-Debate National Survey Miami, September 30, 2004

What Is A Political Party?

1. A Republican edge in terms of self-described interest in the election. 2. Lower levels of self-described interest among younger and Latino

25% Percent of General Voters 20% 15% 10%

US Government Module 3 Study Guide

CHANGING DEMOGRAPHICS AND IMMIGRATION POLITICS IN ARIZONA. March 4, 2014

Political Sophistication and Third-Party Voting in Recent Presidential Elections

Religion and Politics: The Ambivalent Majority

FOR RELEASE APRIL 26, 2018

Indicate the answer choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

MEASURING THE USABILITY OF PAPER BALLOTS: EFFICIENCY, EFFECTIVENESS, AND SATISFACTION

Political Sophistication and Third-Party Voting in Recent Presidential Elections

Behavior and Error in Election Administration: A Look at Election Day Precinct Reports

Characteristics of Poverty in Minnesota

Curriculum Vitae (September 2016)

Counting Ballots and the 2000 Election: What Went Wrong?

REPORT TO PROPRIETARY RESULTS FROM THE 48 TH PAN ATLANTIC SMS GROUP. THE BENCHMARK OF MAINE PUBLIC OPINION Issued May, 2011

January 19, Media Contact: James Hellegaard Phone number:

Transcription:

Intentional Undervotes in Presidential Elections, 1972-2000 Tom W. Smith NORCIUniversity of Chicago December, 2005 GSS Topical Report No. 39

Introduction Voting roll-off or the failure of voters to cast votes for all contests in an election has been a topic of research for a long time (Kimball and Kropf, 2005). The closeness of the 2000 presidential election and especially the difficulties that emerged in counting the presidential vote in Florida increased interest in this topic and refocused attention from roll-off for lesser or downballot contests to looking at unrecorded votes for President at the top of the ballot. Most studies of unrecorded voting (Bullock and Dunn, 1996; Bullock and Hood, 2002; Hansen, 2003; Kimball and Kropf, 2003 & 2005; Kimball Owens, and McAndrew, 2001; Kimball, Owens, and Keeney, 2004; Knack and Kropf, 2003; Kropf and Knack, 2004; Wattenberg, McAllister, and Salvanto, 2000) have utilized official counts and analyzed these by geographic units (e.g. states, counties, precincts) and the aggregate characteristics of these units (e.g. Census characteristics, type of voting mechanism, ballot design, voter registration data, past voting, etc.). A major limitation of these voting-count studies is that they can not study individual-level behavior in general and in particular can not distinguish intentional undervotes from unintentional undervotes. The former occurs when a voter intentionally fails to vote in a contest and the later occurs when a voter intended to cast a valid vote and probably believed that helshe had done so, but no valid vote was recorded due to an error on hisker part or a failure of the voting system. Undervotes are defined in two different ways in the literature. Most frequently it refers to the number of people voting who did not have a vote recorded for a particular context (i.e an office or referendum issue). This could be because voters intentionally failed to vote or attempted to vote, but no valid vote was recorded. When aggregate counts are compared of people voting in the election vs. people voting for each particular contest, nothing further is known except for the total roll-off or undervotes for each contest. However, some times non-votes are distinguished as undervotes (indicating that no valid vote was recorded for the office) and overvotes (indicating that two or more votes were recorded for the same office and thus no valid single vote) (Wolter et al., 2003). This distinction can be made when individual ballots are examined. Data The General Social Surveys (GSSs) are full-probability, in-person surveys of adults living in households. Full technical details are available in Davis, Smith, and Marsden, 2005. The 1973-2004 GSSs contain items on voting in the most recent presidential election (VOTE72-VOTEOO) and whether and for whom a vote for president was cast (PRES72-PRESOO). "Didn't vote for president" was a pre-coded, but unread, response to the question on whom one voted for president. For example, VOTE00 asked "In 2000, you remember that Gore ran on the Democratic ticket against Bush for the Republicans. Do you remember for sure whether or not you voted in that election?" Then, if one indicated voting, PRESOO asked, "Did you vote for Gore or Bush?" "Didn't vote" was a precoded response to PRESOO. Intentional Presidential Undervoting Estimates Most information on intentional presidential undervotes comes from surveys. The GSS

items on the 1972-2000 presidential elections found that 0.4% of the voters in these presidential elections did not vote for president. Knack and Kropf (2003) in their analysis of the 1980-2000 National Election Studies (NES) found that 0.7% said "no" to questions asking voters in the presidential elections of those year, "How about the election for President? Did you vote for a candidate for President?" Knack and Kropf (2003) also examined the 1992 Voter News Service (VNS) exit poll. In response to "In today's election for President, did you just vote for: Bill ClintonIGeorge BusWRoss PerotIOtherlDidn't vote for President," they found that 0.2% said they didn't vote for president and another 0.5% left the item blank. Based on advice from VNS staffers that "many undervoters likely skipped the presidential vote question because they thought it was not applicable to them," they combined these two categories together and found "up to 0.77% intentionally undervoted" in the 1992 VNS. The GSS item probably undercounts intentional undervotes because a) the item does not explicitly offer that as an option and b) the mentioning of the presidential candidates in the lead and follow-up question serves as prompts that would tend to lead some presidential undervoters to misreport voting for a presidential candidate. The NES and VNS items probably get more reliable measures of undervoting because it is an explicit option. Greater accuracy may also occur on the VNS since recall is over just a few minutes rather than over several weeks for the NES and several months to several years on the GSS. However, an analysis of undervoting by elapsed time on the GSS showed no clear decline in reported levels over time. A similar result comes from the NES (Wattenberg, McAllister, and Salvanto, 2000). Finally, the assumption that all missing cases on the VNS represent undervotes is too extreme. Not answering could easily also reflect a desire not disclose ones vote, an inadvertent omission, literacy problems, and other causes. In addition, one state, Nevada, includes the category "None of these candidates" for the presidential contest. In 1996 1.2% selected this explicit undervote option and in 2000 it was 0.5-0.6% (Kimball, Owens, and Keeney, 2004; Kimball, Owens, and McAndrew, 2001; Knack and Kropf, 2003). Given the legitimizing explicitness of this option, one would expect it to generate more intentional undervotes than in the other 49 states where one has to void a straight-party vote (in those states offering this option) and skip over the presidential section on the ballot for an intentional undervote. Characteristics of Intentional Presidential Undervoters Tables 1 and 2 examine the correlates of intentional, presidential undervoting. First, none of the basic demographics (gender, age, and marital status) are related. Second, race, but not Hispanic ethnicity, is associated. Whites and Blacks do not differ in intentional undervoting, but others on race (Asians, American Indians, some Hispanics, etc.) have three-times the undervoting rate (1.3% vs. 0.4%). Third, region of the country, but not community type, is correlated. The individual regional differences are mostly small and do not follow any clear pattern. Fourth, socio-economic status (labor force status, income, and education) has no relationship. Fifth, two other cognitive variables related to level of education (vocabulary score and interviewer's assessment of respondent's understanding of questions) are not associated. Sixth, respondent cooperativeness was related to reporting income, but not to interviewer ratings of respondent

cooperation. Those who refused to reported their household income are twice as likely to report undervoting than those giving income. Seventh, place of residence at age 16 is associated and country of birth has a borderline relationship. For both variables foreigners are twice as likely to undervote as US residents are. Looking at race and foreign residence together shows that others on race who are foreign born or who grew up outside the US have the highest undervoting rates(respective1y 2.1 % and 2.4%). Eighth, information about the news and holding opinion on governmental spending policies are unrelated with undervoting. Ninth, confidence in the executive and legislative branches of the federal government are not associated with undervoting. Tenth, political orientation, but not membership in political groups, is correlated with undervoting. Undervoting is greatest among independents with no partisan leanings (1.1 %) and next highest among those identifying with third parties (0.8%). Strength of partisanship does not seem to matter. Likewise, in a relationship of borderline statistical significance, the moderates have marginally higher undervoting (0.6%) and those with more extreme ideological positions have the lowest undervoting (0.2-.0.3%). Knack and Kropf (2003) argue that "political alienation... the lack of a candidate who represents their interests" may explain their greater undervoting by independents. Alternatively, ambivalence may be the explanation. Most independents with no partisan leaning are equally poised between the major parties and nothing may tip them towards one major, presidential candidate over the other. This equipositioning may lead to indecision and intentionally not voting for president. Summary National estimates are that about 1.8% to 2% of voters in presidential elections have no recorded vote. A substantial fiaction of that total, about one-quarter to one-third, represents intentional undervotes. They would make an even larger share of unrecorded votes that involved the absence of any vote rather than negating overvotes. For example, in the 2000 presidential election in Florida overvotes were 1.9% of the total votes and undervotes were 1.O% (Wolter et al., 2003). The limited research to date on intentional undervoting fiom the NES (Wattenberg, McAllister, and Salvanto, 2000), the VNS (Knack and Kropf, 2003), and the GSS (here), concur that undervoting is greater among independents and least among partisans. This is also supported by the GSS analysis of political ideology. The GSS also finds that third-party adherents are less likely to vote and this result is backed by the aggregate-level finding that undervoting was greater in 2000 in states where Nader was not on the ballot (Kimball, Owens, and McAllister, 2001 ; Kimball, Owens, and Keeney, 2004). It is also consistent with aggregate-level results that show less presidential undervoting when more candidates are on the ballot (Knack and Kropf, 2003; Kropf and Knack, 2004). The studies also indicate that while there are no differences between Whites and Blacks and between Hispanics and non-hispanics, but that members of other races are less likely to vote. GSS analysis further indicates that this is at least in part due to members of this racial group not being born and/or raised in United States. People in this group may both find candidates of their own ethno-racial group non-existent in presidential contests and rare in other top-level contests and be less familiar with the American political system. But the studies disagree on other points. The VNS analysis showed more intentional

undervoting among older voters and those with lower incomes, but the NES and GSS studies found no relationship with age or income. The NES study of congressional, intentional undervoting found that lack of involvement and interest in the congressional races were related to more undervoting, but none of the measures of general political interest or information on the GSS were associated with presidential undervoting. The VNS study did not examine such variables. The individual-level, intentional voting studies do agree in failing to show some of the relationships commonly found in aggregate-level studies (Bullock and Dunn, 1996; Bullock and Hood, 2002; Hansen, 2003; Kimball and Kropf, 2003 & 2005; Kimball Owens, and McAndrew, 2001; Kimball, Owens, and Keeney, 2004; Knack and Kropf, 2003; Kropf and Knack, 2004; Wattenberg, McAllister, and Salvanto, 2000). None show greater undervoting by Blacks or the less educated. This may indicate that being Black and having less education is primarily related to unintentional undervoting. This is consistent with aggregate-level results that show that racial differences sometimes disappear with the voting system controlled for (Kropf and Knack, 2004). Most of the explanations for intentional undervoting voting (voter fatigue, lack of interest, lack of information; see Coupe and Noury, 2004; Kimball and Kropf, 2005; Kimball, Owens, and McAndrew, 2001; Knack and Kropf, 2003; Kropf and Knack, 2004) operate better to account for the high roll-off for down-ballot contests than for presidential undervoting. The most consistently supported explanations involve greater intentional presidential undervoting by 1) Independents which probably reflects both less attachment to any of the candidates and indecision between equidistant alternatives, 2) third-party supporters who may not cast a vote simply because their party's candidate is not on the ballot, and 3) members of the smallest racial groups (not White, Black, or Hispanic) and perhaps especially those born and/or raised outside the US.

References Bullock, Charles S., I11 and Dunn, Richard E., "Election Roll-Off: A Test of Three Explanations," Urban Affairs Review, 32 (1 996), 71-86. Bullock, Charles S., I11 and Hood, M.V., "One Person - No Vote; One Vote; Two Votes: Voting Methods, Ballot Types, and Undervote Frequency in the 2000 Presidential Election," Social Science Quarterly, 83 (2002), 981-992. Coupe, Tom and Noury, Abdul G., "Choosing Not to Choose - On the Link between Information and Abstention," Economics Letters, 84 (2004), 26 1-265. Davis, James A.; Smith, Tom W.; and Marsden, Peter V., General Social Surveys Cumulative Codebook: 1972-2004. Chicago: NORC, 2005. Hansen, B.E., "Recounts from Undervotes: Evidence from the 2000 Presidential Election," Journal of the American Statistical Association, 98 (2003), 292-298. Kimball, David and Kropf, Martha, "Ballot Design and Unrecorded Votes in the 2002 Midterm Election," Paper presented to the American Political Science Association, Philadelphia, August, 2003. Kimball, David C. and Kropf, Martha, "Ballot Design and Unrecorded Votes on Paper-Based Ballots," Public Opinion Quarterly, 69 (2005), 508-529. Kimball, David C.; Owens, Chs; and McAndrew, Katherine, "Who's Afraid of an Undervote?" Paper presented to the Southern Political Science Association, Atlanta, November, 2001. Kimball, David C.; Owens, Chris T.; and McAndrew, Katherine, "Unrecorded Votes and Political Representation," in Counting Votes: Lessons from the 2000 Presidential Election in Florida, edited by Robert P. Watson. Gainesville: University Press of Florida, 2004 Knack, Stephen and Kropf, Martha, "Voided Ballots in the 1996 Presidential Election: A Country-Level Analysis," Journal of Politics, 65 (2003), 881-897. Kropf, Martha A. And Knack, Stephen, "Balancing Competing Interests: Voting Equipment in Presidential Elections," in Counting Votes: Lessons from the 2000 Presidential Election in Florida, edited by Robert P. Watson. Gainesville: University Press of Florida, 2004 Wattenberg, Martin P.; McAllister, Ian; and Salvanto, Anthony, "How Voting is Like an SAT Test: An Analysis of American Voter Rolloff," American Politics Ouarterly, 28 (2000), 234-250.

Wolter, Kirk et al., "Reliability of the Uncertified Ballots in the 2000 Presidential Election in Florida," American Statistician, 57 (February, 2003), 1-14.

Table 1 Correlates of Not Voting for President Among Voters VariablesIGS S Mnemonic Prob. GenderIS EX AgeIAGE Marital StatusIMARITAL RegiodREGION.016 Community TypeISRCBELT.I86 Labor-Force StatusNRKSTAT.275 IncomeIREALINC.429 EducatiodDEGREE.793 Vocabulary TestNORDSUM.656 ComprehensiodCOMPREND.914 CooperatiodCOOP.962 Refused IncomeIINCOME...INCOM98.050 Region at Age 161REGI 6 Born in USAIBORN Newspaper readership/news.900 Don't Knows on Spending Itemsb.868 Confidence in Congress/CONLEGIS.228 Confidence in Executive BranchICONFED.962 Party IdentificatiodPARTYID.OOO Political Ideology/POLVIEWS.083 Member of Political GroupIMEMPOLIT.940 Personal HappinessIHAPPY.829 Financial SatisfactiodSATFIN.293 "Any mention of Hispanic ancestors of ETH1, ETH2, ETH3, or ETHNIC. b~umber of Don't Know responses to the 15 spending priority items, NATSPAC, etc.

Table 2 % Not Voting for President Race White Black Other Region of Residence at Age 16 Foreign USA Place of Birth USA Not USA Region New England Mid Atlantic South Atlantic East North Central West North Central East South Central West South Central Mountain Pacific Refused Income Gave Refused Party Identification Strong Democrat Democrat Democratic Leaning Independent Republican Leaning Republican Strong Republican Other

Table 2 (continued) % Not Voting for President Political Ideology Extreme Liberal Liberal Slight Liberal Moderate Slight Conservative Conservative Extreme Conservative