Libertarians and the Authoritarian personality

Similar documents
Chasing the Elusive Left-Wing Authoritarian: An Examination of Altemeyer s Right-Wing. Authoritarianism and Left-Wing Authoritarianism Scales

Like many other concepts in political science, the notion of radicalism harks back to the

Avoiding the Pitfalls of Politicized Psychology

The structure of social values: Validation of Rokeach's two-value model

Sample. The Political Role of Freedom and Equality as Human Values. Marc Stewart Wilson & Christopher G. Sibley 1

Submitted in partial fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Arts (Psychological Research) of Rhodes University

Authoritarianism and Conservatism:

Confusing terms: Liberals, Liberalism, and Libertarians

Politics in the United States

Communist and Post-Communist Studies

Liberals (aka the Left)

Your OWN Ideology. Where do you stand? Mr. R. Montgomery Centre High Campus

Understanding China s Middle Class and its Socio-political Attitude

A psycho-political profile of moderates and left-wing and right-wing extremists. Alain Van Hiel

Politics between Philosophy and Democracy

Political Conservatism, Rigidity, and Dogmatism in American Foreign Policy Officials: The 1966 Mennis Data

The Antidemocratic Personality Revisited: A Cross-National Investigation of Working-Class Authoritarianism

CHAPTER 1: Introduction: Problems and Questions in International Politics

1. The Relationship Between Party Control, Latino CVAP and the Passage of Bills Benefitting Immigrants

PUBLIC OPINION POLL ON RIGHT WING EXTREMISM IN SLOVAKIA

Attitudes towards influx of immigrants in Korea

SOME PROBLEMS WITH DEFINITION AND PERCEPTION OF EXTREMISM WITHIN SOCIETY

Authoritarian orientations in Thailand

The political psychology of UKIP: Personality factors, authoritarianism and support for right-wing populist parties in Britain.

Chapter One: The Democratic Republic

Political Beliefs and Behaviors

Personality in politics

PEACE AND CONFLICT: JOURNAL OF PEACE PSYCHOLOGY, 11(3), Copyright 2005, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

AP AMERICAN GOVERNMENT STUDY GUIDE POLITICAL BELIEFS AND BEHAVIORS PUBLIC OPINION PUBLIC OPINION, THE SPECTRUM, & ISSUE TYPES DESCRIPTION

Fascism and Ideology: Italy, Britain and Norway, by Salvatore Garau, by Carlos Manuel Martins

Interview with Victor Pickard Author, America s Battle for Media Democracy. For podcast release Monday, December 15, 2014

The Militant Extremist Mind-Set as a Conservative Ideology Mediated by Ethos of Conflict

Department of Political Science

Why do we have to learn about something that already happened. -- Lessons From History

Analysis of public opinion on Macedonia s accession to Author: Ivan Damjanovski

Can Ideal Point Estimates be Used as Explanatory Variables?

Mr. Baumann s Study Guide Chap. 5 Public Opinion

Examining the underlying complexity of free market beliefs

Agnieszka Pawlak. Determinants of entrepreneurial intentions of young people a comparative study of Poland and Finland

0.1 The World s Continents 1

Polimetrics. Lecture 2 The Comparative Manifesto Project

Political Values and Responses to Fear Messaging: An Australian Perspective VICTORIA UNIVERSITY

Citizenship Education for the 21st Century

From the "Eagle of Revolutionary to the "Eagle of Thinker, A Rethinking of the Relationship between Rosa Luxemburg's Ideas and Marx's Theory

Why Nations Fail A Review

Polimetrics. Mass & Expert Surveys

Understanding Taiwan Independence and Its Policy Implications

Qualities of Effective Leadership and Its impact on Good Governance

Vote Compass Methodology

National Identities and European Integration "From Below': Bringing the People Back In

Power: A Radical View by Steven Lukes

Part. The Methods of Political Science. Part

College of Arts and Sciences. Political Science

MAKING DECISIONS FOR PEOPLE WHO LACK CAPACITY

BETWEEN WORLD WAR I AND WORLD WAR II,

What did we even learn last class?

participation Jonathan Baron Democracy is a human invention, a design that serves certain functions. My hypothesis is that

POSTING CUPE Local 3904 (Unit 1)

PSCI4120 Public Opinion and Participation

CHAPTER 19 MARKET SYSTEMS AND NORMATIVE CLAIMS Microeconomics in Context (Goodwin, et al.), 2 nd Edition

Party Cue Inference Experiment. January 10, Research Question and Objective

November 2018 Hidden Tribes: Midterms Report

Title: The Will to Defend and Personal Values - Ukraine, Estonia and Denmark Author: Kiili, Silva Publication date: July 2016 Category: Analysis

Review : Encounters with the Irrrational: My Story. By Andre E. Haynal, MD

Prof. Bryan Caplan Econ 854

De-coding Australian opinion: Australians and cultural diversity. Professor Andrew Markus

NAME DATE BLOCK. 6) According to the discussion in class, how are interest groups different from political parties? 10) 11)

Political Integration of Immigrants: Insights from Comparing to Stayers, Not Only to Natives. David Bartram

College of Arts and Sciences. Political Science

Chapter One ONE REPUBLIC TWO AMERICAS? Copyright 2014 Cengage Learning

Definition: Property rights in oneself comparable to property rights in inanimate things

August 28, 2012 Political Spectrum

The Political Spectrum

Democracy and Common Valuations

THE rece,nt international conferences

The Political Spectrum

Turnout and Strength of Habits

Analyzing the correlational structure of value-systems by means of open ended questions / 19

Political Socialization. Sept 9/12

Lecture 4: Terrorism and Ideology

Toward an Anthropology of Terrorism. As noted in Chapter 10 of Introducing Anthropology of Religion, terrorism (or any other form of violence)

SUBMISSION FROM THE WELSH LANGUAGE BOARD

Presentation given to annual LSE/ University of Southern California research. seminar, Annenberg School of communication, Los Angeles, 5 December 2003

THE WORKMEN S CIRCLE SURVEY OF AMERICAN JEWS. Jews, Economic Justice & the Vote in Steven M. Cohen and Samuel Abrams

SS: Social Sciences. SS 131 General Psychology 3 credits; 3 lecture hours

Georg Lutz, Nicolas Pekari, Marina Shkapina. CSES Module 5 pre-test report, Switzerland

CONSENSUS DECISION-MAKING

European Politicians on Health and Heart

To Build a Wall or Open the Borders: An Analysis of Immigration Attitudes Among Undergraduate University Students

Do you think you are a Democrat, Republican or Independent? Conservative, Moderate, or Liberal? Why do you think this?

Rhetoric in Economics

Attitudes to global risks and governance

Democracy, and the Evolution of International. to Eyal Benvenisti and George Downs. Tom Ginsburg* ... National Courts, Domestic

Citizenship and morality

Penalties for sexual assault offences

Rhetoric in Economics

Hoffman and Graham note that the word fascist is often used as a term of abuse. FASCISM

II. Freedom and Coercion

Confronting the Nucleus Taking Power from Fascists

Garbage Can Decision Making

Transcription:

Libertarians and the Authoritarian personality by J.J. Ray School of Sociology, University of New South Wales, Australia Like the United States, Australia has a libertarian political party. As in the United States it is plagued with dissension and gets very few votes. Once called the Workers party, it is now called the Progress party. Upon its formation it was immediately identified by the press and much of the community as right-wing and indeed "fascist". Party members and even libertarians who are not party members are also commonly so described. Similar experiences could be related from other parts of the world. Libertarians themselves find it a strange experience to be labelled with the very thing they are devoted to opposing, but the ascription is nonetheless a hard one to dislodge. It stems of course from the habit of seeing all parties as fitting neatly along a single left-right continuum. Since any party's economic policies are a very salient part of its image and since free market policies in general tend to he associated with the political Right, the finding that libertarians advocate extreme market freedom seems to justify an assumption that they are extremely right-wing. In these circumstances, any proof that libertarians can offer to show that libertarianism is not right-wing must be seen as very welcome. There is in the social science literature a very large body of such proof. This paper will review some of it. A reason why the proof might not be well-known to many libertarians is that it goes under the label of the antonym to libertarianism- "authoritarianism". What the literature shows, then, is that it is authoritarianism-not libertarianism-which is characteristically "right-wing." Advocates of individual liberty tend, on the whole, to be "left-wing." The literature starts out with the now-famous book by Adorno, Frenkel- Brunswik, Levinson and Sanford, The Authoritarian Personality. This book had its genesis in an attempt by these four Jewish scholars to explain the rise of German Nazism. Most of the research reported in the book, however, was done in California. These authors constructed a "scale" (list) of authoritarian attitudes which they administered to a wide variety of population samples. They found that

40 THE JOURNAL OF LIBERTARIAN STUDIES Winter those who "scored high" on this scale (endorsed most items on the list) tended to be sympathetic to the political Right and in fact showed "prefascist" personalities. Love of authority was fascist, not love of liberty. The book also showed that authoritarian personalities were in important senses "pathological". Opponents of liberty were psychologically inadequate or even "sick". Welcome though these findings may sound to libertarians, they were soon challenged. A follow-up hook by Christie and Jahoda challenged the California findings on both methodological and substantive grounds.? Methodologically, the point was raised that Adorno er al. had included in their list of attitudes only pro-authority items. There were no actual pro-liberty items. One could only express pro-liberty attitudes by rejecting pro-authority statements. Since many people are rather careless in saying "Yes" to questionnaire items, one could never be sure whether a "Yes" response represented genuine agreement or careless agreement. A high scorer could be either simply agreeable or a genuine authoritarian; in such circumstances, one could never be sure whether it was acquiescence which was correlating with right-wing attitudes or whether it was genuine authoritarianism. The substantive point raised against the California studies was that they were simply obviously false. Right-wingers such as Nazis and Fascists may be authoritarians but equally so are Communists such as Mao and Stalin. Authoritarianism was to be found not at one end of the political spectrum but rather at both ends. This would make libertarians the paragons of moderation and compromise. Libertarians, however, are not moderate or compromising in their love for liberty. They are indeed extremists in that respect as judged by general community standards. A new proposal that substantially helped to resolve this dilemma was a long overdue reconceptualization of political allegiance along two dimensions rather than one. This reconceptualization was associated with the names of Rokeach and Eysenck.3 They rightly identified authoritarianismlibertarianism as being at right angles to (unrelated to) the normal radicalconservative dimension of politics. Both authors, however, had proposals for re-naming the authoritarianism-libertarianism dimension. Rokeach wanted to call it open- and closed-mindedness and Eysenck wanted to call it tough-tender-mindedness. However the two dimensions were labelled, the new schema made a lot of sense. Communists and Fascists could be shown to fall at opposite ends of the first dimension (radicalism-conservatism) but at the same end (authoritarian) of the second dimension. Democrats and Republicans on the other hand could he shown to fall also on opposite sides of the radicalismconservatism divide but in the same position on the authoritarianismlibertarianism dimension (half-way between the two). European Liberals and libertarians would fall in the middle on the left-right dimension but

1980 THE AUTHORITARIAN PERSONALlTY 41 towards the libertarian end of the second dimension (with libertarians in fact at the extreme end of the libertarian dimension). Neat as this schema was, however, there proved to be a great deal of difficulty in showing that people's individual attitudes could in fact be ordered in accordance with it. A particularly crushing attack was directed against Eysenck's theory by Christie and by Rokeach and Hanley on the grounds that Eysenck had failed to find a single attitude statement that was purely "tough-minded" (without being at the same time either right- or leftwing).4 Rokeach later had better success in this respect but follow-up work by other researchers tended to show that there was always at least a slight tendency for his "closed-minded" (authoritarian) statements to be more characteristic of the political Right.5 Rokeach's scale (the " D scale) also shared with the Adorno et at. "F' scale, the problem of one-way wording. Again there were no explicitly libertarian items. Three attempts to remedy this problem were made by Ray using Australian data.6 Three new scales were constructed wherein there were equal numbers of authoritarian and libertarian items. On each occasion, there were strong negative correlations between the two sorts of items and on each occasion the total scores on the scales showed strong correlations with political conservatism as measured both by voting preference and by other scales expressing conservative attitudes on particular social and political questions. The beauty of scales with an equal balance between the two sorts of items is that to get a maximum score a person has to say "Yes" to half the questions and "No" to the other half. People who just say "Yes" to everything thus get a low score on half the items and a high score on the other half. The overall score for them is right in the middle where they belongnot at one end as artificially high scorers. The results obtained with balanced scales are then much more trustworthy than results from one-way-worded scales. Thus, at this point, although we have seen that there are theoretical inadequacies in a onedimensional description of political options and although there have been methodological inadequacies in much of the research in the area, the overall conclusion when all these are taken into account is $till the same as that originally drawn by Adorno et a1.-it is authoritarians, not libertarians, who tend to be politically right-wing and fascist. In fact Ray (1973) showed that both by the mechanical/statistical procedures of factor analysis and by the criteria of various historical definitions, the Adorno et a/. "F'scale was indistinguishable from a measure of conservatism.' There are two very important ways, however, in which the Adorno et 01.

42 THE JOURNAL OF LIBERTARIAN STUDIES Winter account has not been shown to be true. First, authoritarians/conservatives can not be shown to be psychologically sick, and, second, authoritarian attitudes can not be shown to go with authoritarian hehaviour. Various measures of authoritarianism have repeatedly been found not to correlate with various measures of maladjustment (including standard clinical indices such as the Eysenck "N scale.8 Examples of such findings can be found in Ray, Masling, Elms, Richek et al, Fraccia et al. and Gaensslen el al.9 Attributes that authoritarianism has been found to correlate with (e.g., rigidity, dogmatism) are obviously not always maladaptive. As "stick-to-it-iveness", such attributes might in some circumstances be, in fact, rather admired.lq The failure of authoritarian attitudes to relate to authoritarian behaviour is, however, a more serious failure of the Adorno et al. account. In fact, to psychologists the attitude/behaviour discrepancy is a familiar phenomenon. It is certainly true in other fields such as racism." Explanations for it differ, but it could have its origin in the desirability of other people's not being able to guess our motives. Since a motive hidden even from ourselves (Freud's "unconscious motivation") is probably the one best hidden of all, real and conscious motivations of opposite character may be quite common. Since not everyone is so devious, however, there are also large numbers of people who do consciously acknowledge their real motivations. Overall, then, there is "orthogonality" between the two. One cannot even guess whether the acknowledged motivation is the real motivation or not. This, however, is only one possible explanation for the phenomenon. Other effects will surely be involved also. The evidence for the unrelatedness of authoritarianjlibertarian attitudes and authoritarianllibertarian behaviour has been extensively summarized before, so it will not be repeated here.12 Since a distinction is necessary between authoritarian attitudes and behaviour, a very obvious question becomes: Given that we have seen authoritarian attitudes to be characteristically conservative, is it also true that those who behave in an authoritarian way are conservative? The evidence on this question is not yet extensive but so far all available results show no relationship between the two whatever.13 People who behave in an authoritarian way are equally likely to be from the Left, the Right or the center. From all the above, then, the conclusion appears to be that a person of libertarian views is likely to have more in common with the political Left than the political Right, but whether in his personal life he will tend to behave in a liberal/permissive way or in an authoritarian/directive way cannot be predicted.

1980 THE AUTHORITARIAN PERSONALITY 43 NOTES I. T. W. Adorno. E. Frenkel-Brunswik, D. J. Levinson, and R. N. Sanford, The Aurhorilorion Personolity (New York: Harper & Row, 1950). 2. R. Christie and M. Jahoda, Sludies in the Scope ond Method of "The Authoritarian Personaliry" (Glencoe, Ill.: The Free Press. 1954). 3. M. Rokeach, The Open ond Closed Mind(New York: Basic Books, 1960); H. J. Eysenck, The Psychology of Politics (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1954). 4. R. Christie, "Eysenck's Treatment of the Personality of Communists," Psyehologiral Bullelin 53 (1956): 41 1-38; M. Rokeach and C. Hanley, "Care and Carelessness in Psychology," Psychologicol Bulletin 53 (1956): 183-86. 5. D. J. Hanson, "Dogmatism and Political Ideology," Journal of Humon Relnrions 18 (1970): 995-1002. 6. J. J. Ray, "An 'Attitude to Authority' Scale," Ausrralion Psychologist 6 (1971): 31-50; idem.. "Non-Ethnocentric Authoritarianism." Ausrralion and New Zeolond Journal of Sociology 8 (1972): 96-102; and idem. "A New Balanced 'F' Scale-and Its Relation to Social Class," Ausrrolian Psychologirt 7 (1972): 155-66. 7. Ray, "Conservatism, Authoritarianism and Related Variables: A Review and an Empirical Study," in G. D. Wilson, ed., The Psychology of Conservofisrn(London: Academic Press, 1973). chap. 2. 8. H. J. Eysenck, The Biological Basis of Personolity (Springfield, Ill.: Thomas, 1967). 9. Ray, "Attitude to Authority"; idem., "Militarism, Authoritarianism, Neuroticism and Antisocial Behavior," Journol of Con/lier Resolulion 16 (1972): 31940; M. Masling, "How Neurotic is the Authoritarian? Journal of Abnormal and Social Psyeho1og.v 49 (1954): 316-18; A. C. Elms, "Those Little Old Ladies in Tennis Shoes are No Nuttier Than Anyone Else: It Turns Out." Psychology Todoy 3 (1970): 278; H. G. Richek, C. D. Mayo, and H. B. Puryear, "Dogmatism, Religiosity and Mental Health in College Students," Mental Hygiene 54 (1970): 572-74; J. Fraccia, G. Sheppard, J. Pintyr, J. Crovella, and S. Merlis, "Personal Adjustment and Authoritarian Attitudes Towards the Mentally Ill," Psychological Reporrs 31 (1972): 483-86; and H. Gaensslen, F. May, and F. Woelpert, "Relation Between Dogmatism and Anxiety," Psychological Reports 33 (1973): 955-58. 10. Also see J. J. Ray and J. Martin, "How Desirable is Dogmatism? Ausrrolian ond New Zeolond Journal of Sociologv 10 (1974): 143-44. 11. See Ray, "Ethnocentrism: Attitudes and Behavior," Ausrrnlion Quarterly43 (1971): 89-97. 12. See Ray, "Do Authoritarians Hold Authoritarian Attitudes? Human Relorions 29 (1976):.. 707-725~ 13. Ray, "Does Authoritarianism of Personality Go with Conservatism?" Auslrolian Journalof Psychology 31 (1979): 9-14.