What Does "Unwilling" to Impose the Death Penalty Mean Anyway? Another Look at Excludable Jurors

Similar documents
The Role of Death Qualification in Venirepersons Evaluations of Aggravating and Mitigating Circumstances in Capital Trials

Death Penalty Beliefs and Jurors' Responses to Aggravating and Mitigating Circumstances in Capital Trials

Religious Beliefs, Motion for Voir Dire on Sentence Length, and Motion for Voir

Steps in the Process

Do Capital Jurors Understand Mitigation? Why mitigation? 4/13/2011. Aggravation vs. Mitigation

Psychological Reports, 1982, 50, Psychological Reports 1982

THE CONSTITUTIONALITY OF DEATH QUALIFIED JURIES. Charles Chastain (University of Arkansas, Little Rock) Introduction

RECENT RESEARCH. Saks, M. and Marti, M. (1997) "A Meta-Analysis Jury Size", Law and Human Behavior 21:

Death-Qualification and the Fireside Induction

Sentencing: The imposition of a criminal sanction by a judicial authority. (p.260)

Chapter 9. Sentencing, Appeals, and the Death Penalty

Chapter 6 Sentencing and Corrections

1. If several suspected offenders are involved in the same criminal. accusation or indictment, no defense attorney shall be allowed to represent

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

Intro to Death Penalty Voir Dire

Fifth, Sixth, and Eighth Amendment Rights

Law Day 2005 Judges or Attorney Lesson: To Speak the Truth

Discretion in Capital Sentencing Instructions: Guided or Misguided?

SUPREME COURT OF THE UNITED STATES

CAUSE NUMBER 00 THE STATE OF TEXAS IN THE COUNTY CRIMINAL V. COURT AT LAW NUMBER 00 DEFENDANT OF HARRIS COUNTY, TEXAS

HOW DO THE FIFTH, SIXTH, AND EIGHTH AMENDMENTS PROTECT RIGHTS WITHIN THE JUDICIAL SYSTEM?

Questioning Capital Punishment: Law, Policy, and Practice James R. Acker

A Deadly Bias: First-Time Offenders and Felony Murder

Ross v. Oklahoma: A Reversal of the Reversible- Error Standard in Death-Qualification Cases

SS.7.C.3.3 and SS.7.C.3.8 Judicial Branch: Article III

Lockhart v. McCree: Death Qualification of Jury Prior to Guilt Phase of Bifurcated Capital Trial Held Constitutional

Felony Cases. Police Investigation. Associate Circuit Court. Felony Versus Misdemeanor

RIGHTS OF THE ACCUSED. It is better to allow 10 guilty men to go free than to punish a single innocent man.

Landmark Case MANDATORY MINIMUM SENTENCE FOR MURDER R. v. LATIMER

CALIFORNIA v. BROWN SUPREME COURT OF THE UNITED STATES. 479 U.S. 538; Argued December 2, 1986, Decided January 27, 1987

Criminal Justice in America CJ Chapter 10 James J. Drylie, Ph.D.

CRJ Social Science in Law Fall 2002 Study Guide 3 Dr. Karu Hangawatte

Chapter 12 CAPITAL PUNISHMENT. Introduction to Corrections CJC 2000 Darren Mingear

Overview of the Jury System. from the Perspective of a Korean Attorney. From the perspective of a Korean attorney, the jury system

in Juvenile Court: The Role of the District Attorney Is the Juvenile Court Becoming Just Like Adult Court? By INGER J. SAGATUN and LEONARD P.

PREFACE. The Constitution Project xv

Morgan v. Illinois: The Right to Balance Capital Sentencing Juries as to Their Views on the Death Sentence Is Finally Granted to Defendants

JURY SELECTION QUESTIONS

NC General Statutes - Chapter 15A Article 100 1

Report on Jury Selection Study

Supreme Court of the United States

IN THE SUPREME COURT OF FLORIDA. To the Chief Justice and Justices of the Supreme Court of Florida:

A GUIDEBOOK TO ALABAMA S DEATH PENALTY APPEALS PROCESS

Jan Hoth, for appellant. Meredith Boylan, for respondent. Innocence Project, Inc.; Legal Aid Society et al., amici curiae.

IN THE SUPREME COURT OF FLORIDA CASE NO. SC DIETER RIECHMANN, Appellant, STATE OF FLORIDA, Appellee.

Present: Carrico, C.J., Compton, Stephenson, Lacy, Hassell, and Keenan, JJ., and Whiting, Senior Justice

UNIT 1: GUILT AND LIABILITY

Introduction to the Presentations: The Path to an Eighth Amendment Analysis of Mental Illness and Capital Punishment

ERRATA SHEET FOR ROBINSON, CRIMINAL LAW: CASE STUDIES & CONTROVERSIES, THIRD EDITION (as of March 25, 2013)

Psychology and Law. I. How are jurors influenced by witnesses, the defendant, and the judge? A. How are jurors influenced by eyewitness testimony?

SULLIVAN v. LOUISIANA. certiorari to the supreme court of louisiana

STATE OF MICHIGAN COURT OF APPEALS

CHAPTER. OPENER- USE YOUR NOTES TO ANSWER THESE REVIEW Q s The Courts: Structure and Participants. Upper Saddle River, NJ 07458

IN THE SUPREME COURT OF FLORIDA. Case No. SC L. C. Case No CFA REPLY BRIEF OF APPELLANT

THE STATE OF NEW HAMPSHIRE SUPREME COURT

Death Penalty. crimes. According to the Supreme Court rulings, the death penalty is not in violation of the

Capital Punishment. The use of the death penalty to punish wrongdoers for certain crimes. Micki ONeal 12/5/2011

Imprisonment is just one of several sentencing options.

REPLY BRIEF OF THE APPELLANT

1. refers to the ability of criminal justice personnel to choose from an array of options or outcomes. Due process Discretion System viability Bias

Joint Committee on Criminal Justice. Richard C. Dieter

The Art of Judging Within a Judges' Panel

Canadian soldiers are entitled to the rights and freedoms they fight to uphold.

Misguided Instructions: Do Jurors Accurately Understand the Law in Death Penalty Trials?

Case 5:06-cr TBR Document 101 Filed 03/21/2008 Page 1 of 11 UNITED STATES DISTRICT COURT WESTERN DISTRICT OF KENTUCKY AT PADUCAH

AGENCY BILL ANALYSIS 2017 REGULAR SESSION WITHIN 24 HOURS OF BILL POSTING, ANALYSIS TO: and

The Effects of the Right to Silence on the Innocent s Decision to Remain Silent

SUPREME COURT OF THE UNITED STATES

James Hamilton, Director of Public Prosecutions, Ireland International Society for the Reform of Criminal Law Conference 15 July 2008, Dublin

Criminal Law - Death Penalty: Jury Discretion Bridled

CHAPTER 14 PUNISHMENT AND SENTENCING CHAPTER OUTLINE. I. Introduction. II. Sentencing Rationales. A. Retribution. B. Deterrence. C.

APPENDIX E. MINORITY REPORT 7.7 Manslaughter

STATE OF OHIO ) CASE NO. CR ) Plaintiff, ) ) vs. ) JOURNAL ENTRY ) ELIJAH FRAZIER ) ) Defendant. )

The court process CONSUMER GUIDE. How the criminal justice system works. FROM ATTORNEY GENERAL JEREMIAH W. (JAY) NIXON

Unit 4 Assessment Amending the Constitution

THE GENERAL ASSEMBLY OF PENNSYLVANIA SENATE RESOLUTION

Court of Appeals of New York, People v. LaValle

A Primacy Effect in Decision-Making by Jurors

STATE OF MICHIGAN COURT OF APPEALS

SUPREME COURT OF THE UNITED STATES

All in Good Faith: N.C. Law and the Good Faith Exception Legal Question of the Week Vol. 4, Number 6 March 25, 2011

In the Supreme Court of Virginia held at the Supreme Court Building in the City of Richmond, on Thursday, the 19th day of January, 2006.

Should Capital Punishment Be Considered Humane or Cruel and Unusual? Capital Punishment

Three Strikes Analysis:

Voir Dire Workshop. Making and Preserving For- Cause Challenges in Voir Dire

Maintaining System Integrity in Capital Cases: The Use of Court-Appointed Counsel to Present Mitigating Evidence When the Defendant Advocates Death

Intended that deadly force would be used in the course of the felony.] (or)

Criminal Justice Public Safety and Individual Rights

State Issue 1 The Neighborhood Safety, Drug Treatment, and Rehabilitation Amendment

Report for the Associated Press: Illinois and Georgia Election Studies in November 2014

STIPULATED JURY INSTRUCTIONS State v. Manny Rayfield Curr County Circuit Court Case No State of New Maine

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at

Appendix: Legal Boundaries Between the Juvenile and Criminal. Justice Systems in the United States. Patrick Griffin

Criminal Case. EQ: What is the sequence of events for a criminal case?

(a) Except as provided in K.S.A Supp and , and amendments thereto, if a

NO IN THE SUPREME COURT OF THE UNITED STATES

RBS SAMPLING FOR EFFICIENT AND ACCURATE TARGETING OF TRUE VOTERS

CERTIFICATION PROCEEDING

STATE V. GRELL: PLACING THE BURDEN ON DEFENDANTS TO PROVE MENTAL RETARDATION IN CAPITAL CASES

International Covenant on Civil and Political Rights 1 Adopted 16 December 1966 Entered into force 23 March 1976

Transcription:

What Does "Unwilling" to Impose the Death Penalty Mean Anyway? Another Look at Excludable Jurors Robert J. Robinson Law and Human Behavior, Vol. 17, No. 4. (Aug., 1993), pp. 471-477. Stable URL: http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0147-7307%28199308%2917%3a4%3c471%3awd%22tit%3e2.0.co%3b2-5 Law and Human Behavior is currently published by Springer. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/journals/springer.html. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is an independent not-for-profit organization dedicated to and preserving a digital archive of scholarly journals. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. http://www.jstor.org Wed Jun 13 17:42:16 2007

Law and Human Behavior, Vol. 17, No. 4, 1993 Research Note What Does "Unwilling" to Impose the Death Penalty Mean Anyway? Another Look at Excludable Jurors Robert J. Robinson* The debate regarding the death qualification of juries usually concerns (a) whether death-qualified jurors have different attitudes and values to excludable jurors, or (b) whether death-qualified juries are more prone to convict. A pivotal question is whether excludable subjects in fact will ever impose the death penalty. Subjects were presented with five grisly murder vignettes. Only 40% of excludable subjects refused to consider the death penalty in all of the cases, with the remaining 60% indicating they would consider the death penalty in one or more of the cases. It is argued that the majority of individuals currently being excluded from capital trial juries based on their reservations about the death penalty actually would impose the death penalty for serious enough offenses and that they should therefore be allowed to serve on such juries. The issue of death qualification of juries in capital punishment cases remains controversial. Most recently, this journal's Adversary Forum published an ongoing discussion between Elliott (1991a, 1991b) and Ellsworth (1991). The predominance of research in this area concentrates on the effect of the exclusion of * Requests for reprints should be sent to Robert Robinson, Harvard Business School, Soldiers Field Park, Boston, Massachusetts 02163. 47 1 0147-730719310800-0471$07.0010O 1993 Plenum Publishing Corporation

472 ROBINSON individuals from juries in capital cases for reason of their opposition to the death penalty. Specifically, by the criteria presented by Cowan, Thompson, and Ellsworth (1984), potential jurors who indicate that they (a) could fairly decide the defendant's guilt or innocence and would fairly follow the judge's instructions in that regard (even if voting guilty meant that the defendant might then go on to receive the death penalty), but (b) who indicate that they are "unwilling" to impose the death penalty themselves in any case, are termed Witherspoon excludable~' and may be excluded from capital cases where the death penalty is a possibility. The rationale for this is that such jurors would not be able to perform all their duties, because if the development is found guilty and the prosecution requested the death penalty, they would be unable to consider that ~ption.~ Those individuals who indicate that they likewise would fairly decide guilt or innocence and who are, in certain instances, in favor of the death penalty (and therefore presumably could, if necessary, sentence the defendant to death) are included in capital case juries and are termed death-qual$ed jurors or, for our present purposes, includables. The research in this area, and therefore the attendant controversy, may be divided into two main areas. The first concerns differences in attitudes and values between excludable jurors and death-qualified jurors. Research has shown deathqualified jurors to be more concerned with crime control and less with due process than excludables, more likely to assume that the defendant is guilty before hearing any evidence presented, less remorseful over a wrongful conviction, and, in general, deviating from excludables on several attitudinal issues concerning issues of law enforcement (Ellsworth, 1991; Fitzgerald & Ellsworth, 1984). The second line of research concerns the empirical question of whether or not death-qualified juries do indeed produce higher conviction rates than do "mixed" juries, where excludable jurors have not been removed. This approach also has a long tradition of research (Haney, 1984; Jurow, 1971; Kalven & Zeisel, 1966). The most provocative and far-reaching of these studies was perhaps that of Cowan et al. (1984). Cowan et al. discovered that under simulated trial conditions, juries comprised entirely of death-qualified individuals were significantly more likely to convict the defendant than juries in which between two and four excludable jurors had been included. They also argued that the diversity of opinion on the mixed juries lead to more vigorous debate, more critical discussion, better recall of facts, and, consequently, a better quality of decision than those of death-qualified juries. Generally the research discussed above has been performed for very pragmatic reasons, the underlying theoretical issues notwithstanding. The American Psychological Association has taken the unusual step in this regard of offering a brief (Bersoff & Ogden, 1987) in which they assert that the process of death qualification does indeed produce a biased jury. This issue is all the more pressing ' Witherspoon v. Illinois (1968). For present purposes, I shall refer to this group as excludables. The particular standard for exclusion varies from state to state and depends on other factors such as whether or not capital crime trials in that state have bifurcated trials and one or two juries for separate verdict and sentencing phases.

ANOTHER LOOK AT EXCLUDABLE JURORS 473 because data indicate that severe bias in the imposition of the death penalty still exists (Foley, 1987). In general, the heat of the debate surrounding the question of death qualification continues to center on the second of these two research approaches. As noted above, there appears to be considerable consensus regarding the question of attitude differences: Death-qualified jurors and excludable jurors differ reliably on any number of issues regarding the purpose of punishment, the rights of the defendant, the standard of proof, the prior likelihood of guilt, and so forth. Where researchers diverge, however, is on the question of the effect of these attitudinal differences. Whereas researchers such as Ellsworth (1991) continue to maintain that such attitudes result in differential conviction rates for death-qualified and mixed juries, others, as presented and summarized by Elliott and Robinson (1991), have been unable to replicate this effect. Though this question is likely to remain controversial for some time, there is a third, and relatively underrepresented area of investigation which concerns the definition of the two groups. Although the Witherspoon standard remains popular among researchers, as Thompson (1989) has pointed out, the so-called Witt standard (Wainright v. Witt, 1985) is now more widely applicable, although how this is being operationalized at the various state and federal levels is still unclear. However, the Witt standard is extremely subjective and unsatisfactory for controlled research: The only satisfactory tool from the perspective of standardization and replication appears to remain the Witherspoon questions. Whether researchers use the Witherspoon questions, the Witt standard, or any other criteria, one underlying, unchallenged assumption appears to remain constant: that those jurors who are excluded from capital trial juries, regardless of the particular exclusion method used, are excluded because they will not vote to impose the death penalty, regardless of the facts of the case. However, what subjects are traditionally asked (certainly for the Witherspoon questions) is whether or not they are "willing to impose the death penalty in any case." However, it is not necessary that we be "willing" before we do something. I pay my taxes, not willingly, but I realize that it needs to be done. The question really is whether subjects could, or would, ever vote for the death penalty. Investigating just this issue, Cox and Tanford (1989) found that 65% of excludables were willing to consider imposing the death penalty in one or more cases when asked to review 16 different murder vignettes. Cox and Tanford's finding are powerfully provocative, particularly since they bring into question the entire raison d'&tre for excluding Witherspoon-classified jurors from trials. For this reason, I attempt here, using substantially the same logic as Cox and Tanford, to "rehabilitate" the jury-eligibility of some of these excludable individuals. This study differs from that of Cox and Tanford in design to the extent that my intention was to make it more difficult for subjects to impose the death penalty, providing a more conservative test. Thus most notably (a) I use only five murder vignettes, rather than the 16 used by Cox and Tanford, giving the subjects fewer chances to choose a murder scenario in which they might impose the death penalty; and (b) subjects are asked whether or not they would impose the death

474 ROBINSON penalty in each case, rather than assessing, as Cox and Tanford's subjects did, the "appropriateness" of the death penalty in each case. Like Cox and Tanford, I intend to demonstrate that the group of excludables in fact contains many individuals who, in contrast to the way they have traditionally been understood, are not opposed to the death penalty in all cases, but who simply have an extremely high standard for imposing the ultimate punishment. I shall argue, based on these results, that excluding death penalty opponents from capital trials does not primarily eliminate those individuals who would be unable to function as jurors: Rather, the most obvious effect is to lower the jury's threshold for the imposition of the death penalty. METHOD Subjects Subjects were obtained from two universities in the San Francisco Bay area. All subjects were recruited from introductory psychology classes and received class credit for their participation. A total of 602 subjects participated. Procedure As part of their introductory psychology class requirement, subjects participated in an open "questionnaire day," where a number of experimenters submitted unrelated paper-and-pencil tasks in a precollated package. All subjects received the basic Witherspoon questions (as described by Cowan et al., 1984) early on in their package. Where additional information was collected (as described below), this was included in a separate questionnaire in a different typeface toward the end of the package. The specific secondary questionnaires used five grisly murder vignettes which the subjects read. The vignettes covered a range of crimes: an interracial murder; the abduction, molestation, and murder of children; a professional "hitman"; a serial killer who kidnapped and tortured young women; and a poisoning to benefit from a will. For each vignette, subjects were instructed to imagine that they were on the jury and that the defendant was guilty (the vignettes were written so as to leave no doubt as to the guilt of the defendant). Having been found guilty, the jury now had to deliberate on whether or not to impose the death penalty, which the prosecutor was calling for. Subjects (without discussion with any other subject) indicated their own choice according to the following scale: 1 = I could never vote to impose the death penalty in this specific instance; 2 = I am opposed to the death penalty, but if the rest of the jury felt it was appropriate in this specific instance, I could go along with them; 3 = I might vote for the death penalty in this specific instance; and 4 = I would definitely vote for the death penalty in this specific instance. RESULTS Of the 109 excludable subjects (as classified by their earlier responses to the Witherspoon questions as persons opposed to the death penalty in all cases, but

ANOTHER LOOK AT EXCLUDABLE JURORS 475 who would nevertheless fairly follow the instructions of the judge), only 39.4%, when presented with the five vignettes, responded that they would never vote to impose the death penalty on any of the defendants. The remaining 60.6% of excludables indicated that they would be prepared to go along with the death penalty in at least one case, and 57.8% of excludables indicated that they would go along with the death penalty in more than one of the cases. Indeed, 49.5% of excludables were prepared to possibly impose the death penalty in more than one case, regardless of the position of the rest of the jury, 24.8% of excludables indicated that they would definitely vote to impose the death penalty in at least one case, and 5.5% of excludables indicated that they would impose it in all five cases. By way of contrast, only 1.1% of includables refused to consider the death penalty for any of the cases, while the remaining 98.9% were prepared to consider it for at least one case, and 16.9% indicated that they would impose it in all cases. Even most nullifiers (those subjects who in responding to the Witherspoon questions indicated that they were opposed to the death penalty in all cases and might not obey the judge's instructions in the event of the death penalty being a possibility) were prepared to impose the death penalty. Of the 16,7 steadfastly refused to impose the death penalty in any case, but 9 (56.2%) indicated that they could go along with it in at least one case. Twenty-seven automatic death proponents (ADPs: subjects who indicated that they were in favor of the death penalty and might in fact not obey the judge's instructions in a death-penalty case) were also in the sample, and not a single one of them refused to impose the death penalty in all cases (or even in more than one of the cases). Six (22.2%) of these ADPs indicated that they would vote for the death penalty in all 5 cases. DISCUSSION The findings of Cox and Tanford (1989) were largely replicated, down to a very close match in the total percentage of "rehabilitated" excludables. Clearly, not all subjects who are excluded from capital punishment trials by criteria such as Witherspoon are going to vote for the death penalty. Most of them do, however, appear to be willing to vote for the death penalty in certain instances. These individuals virtually fit the definition of the Witherspoon includable: Someone who would impose the death penalty "in some cases" (Cowan et al., 1984 p. 62). Such cases, as illustrated in this article, are when there has been an extremely cruel crime, an extremely grisly act, just those instances for which the death penalty is supposedly reserved. From the perspective of subjects, to be morally opposed to the death penalty is not the same as refusing to concede that it is sometimes called for. For many people, to be opposed to the death penalty is an abstract philosophical positionwhen faced with specific heinous acts, which alters what Asch (1951) has called the "object of judgment," many of these people are likely to perform the psychological equivalent of saying, "Well, in this case..." While there is undoubt-

476 ROBINSON edly a subset within the excludable group for whom no exceptions will ever be possible, the present results suggest that this may be no more than a third of those individuals currently being excluded from capital punishment cases. It might be argued that the pencil-and-paper nature of this study made it easier for subjects to hypothetically sentence someone to death; on the other hand, the real-life drama of a trial, with grieving friends and family, graphic evidence, and the considerable and underestimated group pressure of the jury room, could make this number of true death-penalty opponents much smaller. Certainly, this is an avenue for further research. Further, the fact that the subjects were college students raises the issue of representativeness. Although this is always of concern, there is no reason in the current study to believe that the effects demonstrated here would be absent in the general public. It is a matter of policy, not science, as to whether or not this group of "rehabilitated excludables" should be allowed onto capital juries and whether or not justice is being harmed by keeping them off. Though the current Supreme Court may not feel that such juries offer a lower standard of justice, it is to be wondered, given the present results, what possible justification there can be, legal, moral, or otherwise, for continuing to exclude prospective jurors simply because they express reservations toward the death penalty. It is not for the Supreme Court, or any other body, to decide that the threshold of imposition should be artificially lowered: Who deserves the death penalty is the decision of a jury which reflects societal values. Removing individuals who would vote for the death penalty, but only in rare instances, may fundamentally alter those critical and subtle shades of value and may be the difference between life and death. REFERENCES Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion ofjudgment. In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, leadership, and men (pp. 177-190). Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press. Bersoff, D. N., & Ogden, D. W. (1987). In the Supreme Court of the United States Lockharr v. McCree: Amicus curiae brief for the American Psychological Association. American Psychologist, 42, 59-68. Cowan, C. L., Thompson, W. C., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1984). The effects of death qualification on jurors' predisposition to convict and on the quality of deliberation. Law and Human Behavior, 8, 53-79. Cox, M., & Tanford, S. (1989). An alternative method ofjury selection. Law andhuman Behavior, 13, 167-183. Elliott, R. (1991a). Response to Ellsworth. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 91-94. Elliott, R. (1991b). Social science data and the APA: The Lockhart brief as a case in point. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 59-76. Elliott, R., & Robinson, R. J. (1991). Death penalty attitudes and the tendency to acquit or convict: Some data. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 389-404. Ellsworth, P. C. (1991). To tell what we know or wait for Godot? Law and Human Behavior, 15,77-90. Fitzgerald, R., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1984). Due process vs. crime control: Death qualification and jury attitudes. Law and Human Behavior, 8, 31-51.

ANOTHER LOOK AT EXCLUDABLE JURORS 477 Foley, L. A. (1987). Florida after the Furman decision: The effect of extralegal factors on the processing of capital offense cases. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 5, 457-465. Haney, C. (1984). On the selection of capital juries: The biasing effect of the death-qualification process. Law and Human Behavior, 8, 121-132. Jurow, G. (1971). New data on the effect of a death-qualified jury on the guilt determination process. Harvard Law Review, 84, 567-61 1. Kalven, H., & Zeisel, H. (1966). The American jury. Boston, MA: Little Brown. Thompson, W. C. (1989). Death qualification after Wainwright v. Witt and Lockhart v. McCree. Law and Human Behavior, 13, 185-216. Wainwright v. Witt, 53 L.W. 4108 (1985). Witherspoon v. Illinois, 391 U.S. 5 10 (1968).