Psychological Science

Similar documents
Now is the time to pay attention

RULE 1.14: CLIENT WITH DIMINISHED CAPACITY

INSTITUTE of PUBLIC POLICY

The Youth Vote in 2008 By Emily Hoban Kirby and Kei Kawashima-Ginsberg 1 Updated August 17, 2009

a rising tide? The changing demographics on our ballots

RULE 1.1: COMPETENCE. As of January 23, American Bar Association CPR Policy Implementation Committee

Admitting Foreign Trained Lawyers. National Conference of Bar Examiners Washington, D.C., April 15, 2016

Mandated Use of Prescription Drug Monitoring Programs (PMPs) Map

RULE 3.1: MERITORIOUS CLAIMS AND CONTENTIONS

Constitution in a Nutshell NAME. Per

RIDE Program Overview

The Impact of Wages on Highway Construction Costs

RULE 2.4: LAWYER SERVING

Candidate Faces and Election Outcomes: Is the Face-Vote Correlation Caused by Candidate Selection? Corrigendum

Uniform Wage Garnishment Act

Governing Board Roster

A Dead Heat and the Electoral College

Mineral Availability and Social License to Operate

RIDE Program Overview

Geek s Guide, Election 2012 by Prof. Sam Wang, Princeton University Princeton Election Consortium

Next Generation NACo Network BYLAWS Adopted by NACo Board of Directors Revised February, 2017

Migrant and Seasonal Head Start. Guadalupe Cuesta Director, National Migrant and Seasonal Head Start Collaboration Office

Breakdown of the Types of Specific Criminal Convictions Associated with Criminal Aliens Placed in a Non-Custodial Setting in Fiscal Year 2015

NATIONAL VOTER REGISTRATION DAY. September 26, 2017

RULE 4.2: COMMUNICATION WITH PERSON REPRESENTED BY COUNSEL

THE POLICY CONSEQUENCES OF POLARIZATION: EVIDENCE FROM STATE REDISTRIBUTIVE POLICY

Election Cybersecurity, Voter Registration, and ERIC. David Becker Executive Director, CEIR

RULE 3.8(g) AND (h):

2016 NATIONAL CONVENTION

Incarcerated Women and Girls

WYOMING POPULATION DECLINED SLIGHTLY

2018 NATIONAL CONVENTION

If you have questions, please or call

Bylaws of the Prescription Monitoring Information exchange Working Group

Trends in Medicaid and CHIP Eligibility Over Time

UNIFORM NOTICE OF REGULATION A TIER 2 OFFERING Pursuant to Section 18(b)(3), (b)(4), and/or (c)(2) of the Securities Act of 1933

CRAIN S CLEVELAND BUSINESS

FSC-BENEFITED EXPORTS AND JOBS IN 1999: Estimates for Every Congressional District

VOCA 101: Allowable/Unallowable Expenses Janelle Melohn, IA Kelly McIntosh, MT

Ballot Questions in Michigan. Selma Tucker and Ken Sikkema

January 17, 2017 Women in State Legislatures 2017

STATISTICAL GRAPHICS FOR VISUALIZING DATA

Supreme Court Decision What s Next

Congressional Districts Potentially Affected by Shipments to Yucca Mountain, Nevada

the polling company, inc./ WomanTrend On behalf of the Center for Security Policy TOPLINE DATA Nationwide Survey among 1,000 Adults (18+)

Historically, state PM&R societies have operated as independent organizations that advocate on legislative and regulatory proposals.

SPECIAL EDITION 11/6/14

2016 us election results

The Progressive Era. 1. reform movement that sought to return control of the government to the people

Oregon and STEM+ Migration and Educational Attainment by Degree Type among Young Oregonians. Oregon Office of Economic Analysis

A contentious election: How the aftermath is impacting education

State Legislative Competition in 2012: Redistricting and Party Polarization Drive Decrease In Competition

Representational Bias in the 2012 Electorate

Reporting and Criminal Records

Promoting Second Chances: HR and Criminal Records

Public and Subsidized Housing as a Platform for Becoming a United States Citizen

14 Pathways Summer 2014

NATIONAL VOTER SURVEY. November 30 December 3, 2017 N = 1,200 respondents (1/3 Landline, 1/3 Cell, 1/3 Internet) margin of error: +/- 2.

The Progressive Era. Part 1: Main Ideas. Write the letter of the best answer. (4 points each)

Admitting Foreign-Trained Lawyers. Professor Laurel S. Terry Penn State Dickinson School of Law Carlisle, Pennsylvania

Background and Trends

Prison Price Tag The High Cost of Wisconsin s Corrections Policies

50 State Survey of Bad Faith Law. Does your State encourage bad faith?

The Law Library: A Brief Guide

Dynamic Diversity: Projected Changes in U.S. Race and Ethnic Composition 1995 to December 1999

State Governments Viewed Favorably as Federal Rating Hits New Low

Epicenter Cities and International Education 17th AIEC Melbourne, Victoria Australia

Background Checks and Ban the Box Legislation. November 8, 2017

New Population Estimates Show Slight Changes For 2010 Congressional Apportionment, With A Number of States Sitting Close to the Edge

House Apportionment 2012: States Gaining, Losing, and on the Margin

By 1970 immigrants from the Americas, Africa, and Asia far outnumbered those from Europe. CANADIAN UNITED STATES CUBAN MEXICAN

RULE 2.10: Judicial Statements on Pending and Impending Cases

RULE 1.16: DECLINING OR TERMINATING REPRESENTATION

Online Appendix. Table A1. Guidelines Sentencing Chart. Notes: Recommended sentence lengths in months.

Washington, D.C. Update

How States Can Achieve More Effective Public Safety Policies

BYLAWS OF THE NATIONAL STUDENT SPEECH LANGUAGE HEARING ASSOCIATION

Effective Dispute Resolution Systems and the Vital Role of Stakeholders

Graduation and Retention Rates of Nonresidents by State

Kansas Legislator Briefing Book 2019

Presentation Outline

Drew Kurlowski University of Missouri Columbia

TABLE OF CONTENTS. Introduction. Identifying the Importance of ID. Overview. Policy Recommendations. Conclusion. Summary of Findings

Federal Education: Of Elections &Politics. Oh, and Policy. Noelle Ellerson December 2014

New Ambassador Orientation. Laura Keivel Manager, Grassroots Advocacy

2017A Financial Summary. $360MM Adjusted EBITDA $1BN Market Capitalization PREMIUM LOCAL NEWS BRANDS NATIONAL PRESENCE

Presentation to the Bakery, Confectionery, Tobacco Workers and Grain Millers' International Union. Paul Lemmon July 26, 2010

Some Change in Apportionment Allocations With New 2017 Census Estimates; But Greater Change Likely by 2020

State and Local Immigration Laws: Recap of 2013 and Outlook for November 22, 2013

Update on State Judicial Issues. William E. Raftery KIS Analyst Williamsburg, VA

2017A Financial Summary. $360MM Adjusted EBITDA $1BN Market Capitalization PREMIUM LOCAL NEWS BRANDS NATIONAL PRESENCE

Charlie Cook s Tour of American Politics

CA CALIFORNIA. Ala. Code 10-2B (2009) [Transferred, effective January 1, 2011, to 10A ] No monetary penalties listed.

ELECTORAL COLLEGE AND BACKGROUND INFO

Trump, Populism and the Economy

Regulating Lawyers in a Global Arena. Conference of Chief Justices Midyear Meeting, Sea Island, Georgia Jan. 28, 2014

Presented by: Ted Bornstein, Dennis Cardoza and Scott Klug

Election 2014: The Midterm Results, the ACA and You

Matt Clark, DHS Adam Rose and Detlof von Winterfeldt, CREATE Chris Soares, John Wirth, Treasury

Some Change in Apportionment Allocations With New 2017 Census Estimates; But Greater Change Likely by 2020

Transcription:

Psychological Science http://pss.sagepub.com/ 9/11, Act II : A Fine-Grained Analysis of Regional Variations in Traffic Fatalities in the Aftermath of the Terrorist Attacks Wolfgang Gaissmaier and Gerd Gigerenzer Psychological Science 2012 23: 1449 originally published online 15 November 2012 DOI: 10.1177/0956797612447804 The online version of this article can be found at: http://pss.sagepub.com/content/23/12/1449 Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: Association for Psychological Science Additional services and information for Psychological Science can be found at: Email Alerts: http://pss.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://pss.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsreprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav >> Version of Record - Dec 14, 2012 OnlineFirst Version of Record - Nov 15, 2012 What is This?

Research Report 9/11, Act II: A Fine-Grained Analysis of Regional Variations in Traffic Fatalities in the Aftermath of the Terrorist Attacks Psychological Science 23(12) 1449 1454 The Author(s) 2012 Reprints and permission: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0956797612447804 http://pss.sagepub.com Wolfgang Gaissmaier and Gerd Gigerenzer Harding Center for Risk Literacy, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany Abstract Terrorists can strike twice first, by directly killing people, and second, through dangerous behaviors induced by fear in people s minds. Previous research identified a substantial increase in U.S. traffic fatalities subsequent to the September 11 terrorist attacks, which were accounted for as due to a substitution of driving for flying, induced by fear of dread risks. Here, we show that this increase in fatalities varied widely by region, a fact that was best explained by regional variations in increased driving. Two factors, in turn, explained these variations in increased driving. The weaker factor was proximity to New York City, where stress reactions to the attacks were previously shown to be greatest. The stronger factor was driving opportunity, which was operationalized both as number of highway miles and as number of car registrations per inhabitant. Thus, terrorists second strike exploited both fear of dread risks and, paradoxically, an environmental structure conducive to generating increased driving, which ultimately increased fatalities. Keywords decision making, risk perception, risk taking, terrorism Received 10/27/11; Revision accepted 4/9/12 Terrorists can strike twice first, by directly killing people, and second, through dangerous behaviors induced by fear in people s minds. In the aftermath of the terrorist attacks on September 11 (9/11), 2001, from October 2001 to September 2002, there were approximately 1,600 more traffic fatalities in the United States than would normally be expected an increase about 6 times the number of people (256) who died in the aircrafts on September 11 (Gigerenzer, 2006). To explain the increase in traffic fatalities, Gigerenzer (2004) proposed the dread hypothesis: People tend to fear dread risks, that is, low-probability, high-consequence events, such as the terrorist attack on September 11, 2001. If [a] Americans avoided the dread risk of flying after the attack and [b] instead drove some of the unflown miles, one [c] would expect an increase in traffic fatalities. (p. 286) This three-part hypothesis was based on a suggestion by Myers (2001) and first tested for the 3 months after 9/11 by Gigerenzer (2004), who found support for each part. Su, Tran, Wirtz, Langteau, and Rothman (2009) divided the United States into three regions and found that fatalities increased solely in the Northeast, in proximity to New York City (NYC), but not in the rest of the country. Here, we report more fine-grained analyses based on data from the 50 individual states plus Washington, D.C. Figure 1a depicts the absolute monthly increase in fatalities per million inhabitants in each state for the 3 months after 9/11. Although traffic fatalities indeed increased in the Northeast, the map clearly shows that substantial increases also occurred in many states more distant from NYC, particularly in the Midwest. We propose that a combined analysis of psychological reactions to terrorism and the structure of the environment can explain these substantial variations. To describe the relation between mind and environment, Simon (1990) coined the metaphor of a scissors whose two blades are the structure of task environments and the computational capabilities of the actor (p. 7). In the case under consideration, fear induced by terrorist attacks is one blade; the other is an environmental structure that allows fear to become manifest in dangerous behavior such as driving instead of flying. Specifically, we consider driving opportunity to be the second blade. We hypothesized that both fear and good driving opportunity are relevant for explaining where driving actually increased in Corresponding Author: Wolfgang Gaissmaier, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Harding Center for Risk Literacy, Lentzeallee 94, 14195 Berlin, Germany E-mail: gaissmaier@mpib-berlin.mpg.de

1450 Gaissmaier, Gigerenzer a Changes: Fatalities WA OR NV CA AK ID AZ UT MT WY CO NM HI ND MN WI SD IA NE IL KS MO OK AR MS TX LA MI IN AL KY TN OH GA WV SC FL PA NY VA NC VT β = 0.42 4ME NH MA RI NYC CT NJ DE MD 0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.5 > 2.5 Change in Fatalities per Million Inhabitants β = 0.59 b CA WA OR AK Highway-System Length NV ID AZ UT MT WY CO NM HI VT ME ND NH MN NY MA SD WI MI RI PA NYC IA CT NE OH NJ IL IN WV VA DE KS MO KY MD NC TN OK AR SC AL GA MS TX LA FL Highway-System Length per 100,000 Inhabitants (Miles) 30 50 70 90 110 > 110 c CA WA OR AK Changes: Miles NV ID AZ UT MT WY CO NM HI 0 10 20 30 50 > 50 β = 0.40 VT ME ND NH MN NY MA SD WI MI RI PA NYC IA CT NE OH NJ IL IN WV VA DE KS MO KY MD NC TN OK AR SC AL GA MS TX LA FL Change in Miles Driven per Inhabitant Proximity to New York City Fig. 1. U.S. maps showing, by state, (a) the change in driving fatalities in the 3 months subsequent to the September 11 terrorist attacks, (b) highway-system length, and (c) the change in the number of miles driven in the 3 months subsequent to the attacks. Also shown are results of regression analyses in which highway-system length and proximity to New York City were predictors of changes in miles driven, which, in turn, predicted changes in driving fatalities.

9/11, Act II 1451 the aftermath of 9/11. We expected that increased driving, in turn, would explain increased traffic fatalities in those areas. As a prerequisite, we tested whether driving actually increased, which Su et al. (2009) contested. Method Overview We analyzed data for the 3 months following the 9/11 attacks (i.e., October to December 2001) and compared them with data for the same period in the 5 previous years (i.e., 1996 to 2000). To rule out the possibility that there was anything unusual about the year 2001 in general, we also investigated the pre-september months in 2001 (i.e., January to August). Except where we indicate otherwise, the data were based on official sources (U.S. Census Bureau, 2002, 2011, n.d.; U.S. Department of Transportation, Federal Highway Administration, 2004, n.d.; U.S. Department of Transportation, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, n.d.). The 50 individual states plus Washington, D.C., were the units of analysis. Measures Changes in miles driven and traffic fatalities. We computed changes (from the previous year) in the numbers of traffic fatalities and miles driven for each of the 50 states plus Washington, D.C., for the years 1996 to 2001. Unlike in previous research, we analyzed absolute changes per inhabitant (miles driven) and absolute changes per million inhabitants (traffic fatalities) rather than relative percentage changes. Absolute changes are more robust than relative changes when the number of observations is small, as it was for small states, particularly regarding fatalities. Furthermore, absolute changes are symmetrical for increases and decreases (e.g., the absolute difference for an increase from 5 to 10 is identical to the absolute difference for a decrease from 10 to 5, whereas the relative changes in this case are +100% and 50%, respectively). For instance, to determine a state s increase in fatalities for the post-september months of 2001 (October December), we calculated the following for each month: fatalities 2001/millions of inhabitants 2001 fatalities 2000/millions of inhabitants 2000; we then averaged the results for these 3 months. As a control, we recalculated each analysis using relative instead of absolute changes; all results remained virtually identical. Fear. We could not measure post-9/11 fear directly, but in agreement with Su et al. (2009), we find it plausible that fear was higher for people who lived closer to NYC, where the major attacks happened. Supporting this assumption, a survey conducted a few days after the attacks showed that people closest to NYC had the highest rate of substantial stress reactions and that the rate of such reactions decreased with distance from NYC (Schuster et al., 2001). To measure proximity to NYC for each state, we used an online mapping and distance tool (http://www.acscdg.com/), entering NYC as the starting point and the name of the state as the end point. Because only NYC was a well-defined point on the map, the tool standardized the choice of one point within each state; these points were approximately in the middle of the states. Driving opportunity. Driving opportunity in each state for the year 2001 was calculated as the length of the state s nationally significant highways defined by inclusion in the National Highway System (NHS) in 2001 divided by the number of the state s inhabitants in 2001. We chose inclusion in the NHS as our criterion because the NHS represents only 4% of the total U.S. roads but 40% of overall traffic and 90% of tourist traffic, and because 90% of the U.S. population lives within 8 km of an NHS road (Slater, 1996). We used length per capita rather than absolute length because absolute length naturally increases with the number of inhabitants and thus is a stronger reflection of state population than of driving opportunity. This variable was skewed; hence, we log-transformed the values. To check the results for robustness, we also operationalized driving opportunity as the number of car registrations per inhabitant. Results Miles driven Across states, the average monthly increase in miles driven per inhabitant in the post-september months of 2001 was 27.2 miles, which was substantially larger than the average increase of 9.9 miles observed in the previous 5 years in those same months, paired-sample t(50) = 3.90, 95% confidence interval (CI) on the difference: [8.4 miles, 26.2 miles], mean difference = 17.3 miles. In 36 of the 51 cases, the change was larger than the average change in the previous 5 years (sign test: Z = 2.80, p =.005). For the pre-september months, the average monthly changes in miles driven in 2001 did not differ from the average changes in the 5 previous years or were even lower, paired-sample t(50) = 1.85, 95% CI on the difference: [ 17.0 miles, 0.7 miles], mean difference = 8.2 miles. Next, we analyzed regional variations in changes in miles driven. Miles driven increased only slightly as a function of proximity to NYC, if at all, r(49) =.19, 95% CI: [.44,.09]. However, miles driven increased substantially with driving opportunity, whether operationalized as highway miles, r(49) =.45, 95% CI: [.20,.65], or as car registrations per inhabitant, r(49) =.42, 95% CI: [.17,.62]. The two drivingopportunity variables were strongly correlated with each other, r(49) =.65, 95% CI: [.46.78], and car registrations per inhabitant did not explain variance beyond that explained by highway miles per inhabitant, so we focus here on the latter measure. It is encouraging that the two variables yielded basically identical results. As distance to NYC and highway miles per inhabitant were correlated with each other, r(49) =.35, we included both

1452 Gaissmaier, Gigerenzer variables in a multiple regression. The overall model resulted in an R 2 value of.35, F(2, 48) = 12.66, p =.00004. The increase in miles driven was greater the closer a state was to NYC (β = 0.40, p =.002) and the more highway miles the state had per inhabitant (β = 0.59, p =.00002; Figs. 1b and 1c). In an exploratory regression model, we separated highway miles per inhabitant into rural and urban miles; rural miles predicted increased driving (β = 0.59, p =.00003), but urban miles did not (β = 0.08, p =.517). Traffic fatalities Across states, the average monthly increase in traffic fatalities per million inhabitants in the post-september months of 2001 was 1.11; in contrast, there was a decrease of 0.24 fatalities on average in those months in the 5 previous years, pairedsample t(50) = 4.64, 95% CI on the difference: [0.76 fatalities, 1.93 fatalities], mean difference = 1.35 fatalities. In 39 of the 51 cases, the change in 2001 was larger than the average change in the previous 5 years (sign test: Z = 3.64, p =.0003). For the pre-september months, the average monthly changes in fatalities per million inhabitants in 2001 did not differ from the average changes in the 5 previous years or were even lower, paired-sample t(50) = 1.32, 95% CI on the difference: [ 0.81 fatalities, 0.17 fatalities], mean difference = 0.32 fatalities. Finally, we analyzed by-state variations in changes in fatalities after 9/11 (Fig. 1a). Fatalities increased with changes in miles driven, r(49) =.42, 95% CI: [.16,.62]. They did not, however, increase substantially with proximity to NYC, r(49) =.11, 95% CI: [.38,.17], or highway miles per inhabitant, r(49) =.19, 95% CI: [.09,.44], and when we controlled for changes in miles driven, these already small correlations approached zero, r(48) =.03 and r(48) =.01, respectively. General Discussion Both miles driven and traffic fatalities increased substantially in the aftermath of 9/11 compared with the last 3 months in the previous 5 years, and did so in many more states than expected by chance. Moreover, these variables did not increase in the pre-september months in 2001, which lends support to the conclusion that the increases after 9/11 were related to the attacks. This finding is inconsistent with Su et al. s (2009) conclusion that miles driven did not increase after 9/11. Their conclusion partly stemmed from a one-way analysis of variance based on nine data points (3 years 3 months). Although the effect size they obtained was substantial (η p 2 =.27; erroneously reported as.37), the analysis did not have sufficient power to detect such an effect given an α of.05 (observed power =.23; Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007). Additionally, Su et al. argued that the post-9/11 increase in miles driven was not pronounced in comparison with historical trends. The literature outside of psychology (e.g., Blalock, Kadiyali, & Simon, 2009; Sivak & Flannagan, 2004), however, backs the finding of increased miles driven reported here and by Gigerenzer (2004, 2006). Taking historical trends into consideration, Blalock et al. even argued that the overall increase in miles driven that they reported might underestimate the 9/11 effect, as their analyses included miles driven in commercial vehicles, and such miles were not expected to increase. Thus, it seems likely that the number of miles driven did increase. Increases in miles driven, however, were not observed uniformly across the country. To explain these regional variations, we analyzed post-9/11 fear (operationalized as proximity to NYC) together with a relevant aspect of environmental structure (driving opportunity, operationalized as highway miles and car registrations per inhabitant). For a complete picture of increased driving following the terrorist attacks, it was not sufficient to consider only post-9/11 fear. Rather, fear and additionally and to an even greater extent driving opportunity explained regional variations in increased driving. Increased driving, in turn, was the best predictor of increased fatalities and where they occurred, a finding consistent with results reported by Blalock et al. (2009) and Sivak and Flannagan (2004). Because the data reported here are correlational by nature, however, caution is warranted in interpreting the observed relationships causally. Even if the 9/11 attacks were an underlying cause of increased driving, fear of dread risk is not the only possible reason for the increase. Blalock, Kadiyali, and Simon (2007) showed that driving could also have increased because new security measures made flying more inconvenient. Furthermore, the assumed substitution of driving for flying is not directly observable. Consistent with such a substitution, however, is the finding that post-9/11 out-of-state traffic fatalities increased more than home-state traffic fatalities, which suggests that the fatalities occurred on longer-distance trips potentially suited to replace air travel (Blalock et al., 2009; but see Sivak & Flannagan, 2004). It also fits the substitution hypothesis that rural but not urban highway-system miles predicted where miles driven increased, and that driving particularly increased on rural interstate highways (Gigerenzer, 2004). Finally, there is convergent evidence for the importance of driving opportunity for predicting the substitution: After the terrorist attacks in Madrid on March 11, 2004, Spaniards reacted similarly to Americans, experiencing dread-risk fear and reducing their use of trains. But unlike Americans, they did not increase their driving, so that traffic fatalities did not increase (López-Rousseau, 2005). López-Rousseau speculated that this might have happened because there is less of a car culture in Spain than in the United States (p. 427). Data for a measure of car culture that predicted variations in increased driving in our study the number of car registrations per inhabitant support this speculation. The number of car registrations per 1,000 inhabitants was as high as 808 in the United States in 2001, but only 586 in Spain in 2004

9/11, Act II 1453 (Eurostat, 2011a, 2011b). 1 We believe that the assumed causal model of how and where the 9/11 attacks indirectly led to additional fatalities is at least plausible in light of the evidence, taken together. Nevertheless, it has been reported that the increase in traffic fatalities was larger than would be expected if it were based purely on increased driving (Blalock et al., 2009). Su et al. (2009) presented evidence that a decrease in driving quality as a consequence of stress could account for this gap. Similar effects of attack-induced stress on traffic fatalities have been demonstrated in Israel (Stecklov & Goldstein, 2004). Although our results do not provide evidence for such a mechanism, such as a direct link between proximity to NYC and increased traffic fatalities, they also do not preclude it. Detailed analyses of driving quality are beyond the scope of this report, but we checked one straightforward yet powerful indicator of driving quality: If driving quality decreased after 9/11, there should have been more fatalities per mile driven. Indeed, traffic fatalities per mile driven increased, on average, by 4.2% in the post- September months in 2001, but decreased by 1.3% and 5.4% in the same months in 1999 and 2000, respectively. Moreover, the data are consistent with Su et al. s assertion of particularly increased stress in the Northeast, as the post-9/11 increase in traffic fatalities per mile driven was 10.7% in that region. Ten years after 9/11, the news focused again on the physical impact of terrorism, paying little heed to terrorists second strike, the psychological Act II of 9/11. A closer look at the indirect damage in the form of increased traffic fatalities, however, shows that it is important to fight the effects of terrorism in people s minds. Fear caused by terrorism can initiate potentially dangerous behaviors, such as driving more miles or driving less carefully. Understanding fear, however, is not enough. Rather, in order to foresee where secondary effects of terrorism will strike fatally, one also needs to consider environmental structures that allow fear to become manifest in dangerous behaviors, such as driving instead of flying. Acknowledgments We thank Birgit Silberhorn for her help; Alexander Rothman for providing details of Su, Tran, Wirtz, Langteau, and Rothman s (2009) analyses; Juliane Kämmer for her comments; Henry Brighton for suggesting car registrations per inhabitant as another operationalization of driving opportunity; Steven Jessberger from the U.S. Department of Transportation for providing data on monthly vehiclemiles of travel from 1989 through 2005 in each state; and Rona Unrau for editing the manuscript. Special thanks go to the journal s Editor, Robert Kail, who, with a comment on a previous (and very different) version of the manuscript, actually inspired us to systematically analyze regional variations in the first place. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. Note 1. We did not compare highway-system miles between the United States and Spain because the NHS (United States) and the statistical information service of the European Commission, Eurostat (Spain), define highway ( motorway, in the case of Eurostat) differently. References Blalock, G., Kadiyali, V., & Simon, D. (2007). The impact of post- 9/11 airport security measures on the demand for air travel. Journal of Law & Economics, 50, 731 755. Blalock, G., Kadiyali, V., & Simon, D. (2009). Driving fatalities after 9/11: A hidden cost of terrorism. Applied Economics, 41, 1717 1729. Eurostat. (2011a). Population at 1 January: Persons (tps00001) [Data file]. Retrieved from http://epp.eurostat.ec.europa.eu/tgm/ table.do?tab=table&;init=1&language=en&pcode=tps00001& plugin=1 Eurostat. (2011b). Stock of vehicles by category and NUTS 2 regions (tran_r_vehst) [Data file]. Retrieved from http://appsso.eurostat.ec.europa.eu/nui/show.do?dataset=tran_r_vehst&;lang=en Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175 191. Gigerenzer, G. (2004). Dread risk, September 11, and fatal traffic accidents. Psychological Science, 15, 286 287. Gigerenzer, G. (2006). Out of the frying pan into the fire: Behavioral reactions to terrorist attacks. Risk Analysis, 26, 347 351. López-Rousseau, A. (2005). Avoiding the death risk of avoiding a dread risk: The aftermath of March 11 in Spain. Psychological Science, 16, 426 428. Myers, D. G. (2001). Do we fear the right things? Observer, 14, 3. Schuster, M. A., Stein, B. D., Jaycox, L. H., Collins, R. L., Marshall, G. N., Elliott, M. N.,... Berry, S. H. (2001). A national survey of stress reactions after the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. New England Journal of Medicine, 345, 1507 1512. Simon, H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 1 19. Sivak, M., & Flannagan, M. J. (2004). Consequences for road traffic fatalities of the reduction in flying following September 11, 2001. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 7, 301 305. Slater, R. (1996). The National Highway System: A commitment to America s future. Public Roads, 59. Retrieved from http://www.fhwa.dot.gov/publications/publicroads/96spring/p96sp2.cfm Stecklov, G., & Goldstein, J. R. (2004). Terror attacks influence driving behavior in Israel. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, 14551 14556. Su, J. C., Tran, A. G. T. T., Wirtz, J. G., Langteau, R. A., & Rothman, A. J. (2009). Driving under the influence (of stress): Evidence of a regional increase in impaired driving and traffic fatalities after the September 11 terrorist attacks. Psychological Science, 20, 59 65. U.S. Census Bureau. (2002). Table CO-EST2001-12-00: Time series of intercensal state population estimates: April 1, 1990 to April 1,

1454 Gaissmaier, Gigerenzer 2000 [Data file]. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/popest/ data/intercensal/st-co/files/co-est2001-12-00.pdf U.S. Census Bureau. (2011). Preliminary annual estimates of the resident population for the United States, regions, states, and Puerto Rico: April 1, 2000 to July 1, 2010 (NST-PEST2010-01) [Data file]. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/popest/research/ eval-estimates/eval-est2010.html U.S. Census Bureau. (n.d.). Table 1097. State motor vehicle registrations, 1990 to 2008, motorcycle registrations and licensed drivers by state: 2008 [Data file]. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/compendia/statab/2011/tables/11s1097.xls U.S. Department of Transportation, Federal Highway Administration. (2004, June). National highway system length 2001: Miles by functional system. Retrieved from http://www.fhwa.dot.gov/ ohim/hs01/hm41.htm U.S. Department of Transportation, Federal Highway Administration. (n.d.). Monthly vehicle-miles of travel 1989 2005 [Data file]. (Available from the author; for further information, see http:// www.fhwa.dot.gov/policyinformation/travel_monitoring/tvt.cfm) U.S. Department of Transportation, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, National Center for Statistics and Analysis. (n.d.). [Accident data files]. Retrieved from ftp://ftp.nhtsa.dot.gov/fars/