A Report on the Social Network Battery in the 1998 American National Election Study Pilot Study. Robert Huckfeldt Ronald Lake Indiana University

Similar documents
Social Capital, Social Networks, and Political Participation

MEASUREMENT OF POLITICAL DISCUSSION NETWORKS A COMPARISON OF TWO NAME GENERATOR PROCEDURES

Turnout and Strength of Habits

1. The Relationship Between Party Control, Latino CVAP and the Passage of Bills Benefitting Immigrants

Modeling Political Information Transmission as a Game of Telephone

Disagreeing About Disagreement: How Conflict in Social Networks Affects Political Behavior

Wisconsin Economic Scorecard

UTS:IPPG Project Team. Project Director: Associate Professor Roberta Ryan, Director IPPG. Project Manager: Catherine Hastings, Research Officer

Chapter 6 Online Appendix. general these issues do not cause significant problems for our analysis in this chapter. One

This journal is published by the American Political Science Association. All rights reserved.

Learning from Small Subsamples without Cherry Picking: The Case of Non-Citizen Registration and Voting

Statewide Survey on Job Approval of President Donald Trump

Disagreeing About Disagreement: How Conflict in Social Networks Affects Political Behavior

Chapter 14. The Causes and Effects of Rational Abstention

BELIEF IN A JUST WORLD AND PERCEPTIONS OF FAIR TREATMENT BY POLICE ANES PILOT STUDY REPORT: MODULES 4 and 22.

Public Opinion and Government Responsiveness Part II

ONLINE APPENDIX for The Dynamics of Partisan Identification when Party Brands Change: The Case of the Workers Party in Brazil

Colorado 2014: Comparisons of Predicted and Actual Turnout

Understanding Taiwan Independence and Its Policy Implications

Facilitating Communication Across Lines of Political Difference: The Role of Mass Media

Online Appendix 1: Treatment Stimuli

Supplemental Appendices

UNDERSTANDING TAIWAN INDEPENDENCE AND ITS POLICY IMPLICATIONS

ABSENTEE VOTING, MOBILIZATION, AND PARTICIPATION

COMMUNICATION NETWORKS, COMPLEX INTERDEPENDENCE, AND THE SURVIVAL OF DISAGREEMENT AMONG CITIZENS

Report for the Associated Press. November 2015 Election Studies in Kentucky and Mississippi. Randall K. Thomas, Frances M. Barlas, Linda McPetrie,

A Vote Equation and the 2004 Election

Proposal for the 2016 ANES Time Series. Quantitative Predictions of State and National Election Outcomes

Table A.1: Experiment Sample Distribution and National Demographic Benchmarks Latino Decisions Sample, Study 1 (%)

Non-Voted Ballots and Discrimination in Florida

AmericasBarometer Insights: 2014 Number 106

Immigrant Legalization

POLI 300 Fall 2010 PROBLEM SET #5B: ANSWERS AND DISCUSSION

democratic or capitalist peace, and other topics are fragile, that the conclusions of

To what extent do elected politicians keep the

DATA ANALYSIS USING SETUPS AND SPSS: AMERICAN VOTING BEHAVIOR IN PRESIDENTIAL ELECTIONS

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES WHAT DO UNIONS DO... TO VOTING? Richard B. Freeman. Working Paper 9992

Jeffrey M. Stonecash Maxwell Professor

Educated Preferences: Explaining Attitudes Toward Immigration In Europe. Jens Hainmueller and Michael J. Hiscox. Last revised: December 2005

Party Polarization, Revisited: Explaining the Gender Gap in Political Party Preference

CALTECH/MIT VOTING TECHNOLOGY PROJECT A

CSES Module 5 Pretest Report: Greece. August 31, 2016

Economic Context and Americans Perceptions of Income Inequality n

Content Analysis of Network TV News Coverage

GW POLITICS POLL 2018 MIDTERM ELECTION WAVE 1

Jim Justice Leads in Race for West Virginia Governor

Explaining Modes of Participation

Minority Party Loyalty Under Challenge

national congresses and show the results from a number of alternate model specifications for

The Costs of Immigration to Taxpayers: Analytical and Policy Issues

Vote Likelihood and Institutional Trait Questions in the 1997 NES Pilot Study

The National Citizen Survey

Benefit levels and US immigrants welfare receipts

Biases in Message Credibility and Voter Expectations EGAP Preregisration GATED until June 28, 2017 Summary.

Experiments in Election Reform: Voter Perceptions of Campaigns Under Preferential and Plurality Voting

MARJORIE DANNENFELSER, SUSAN B. ANTHONY LIST KEY FINDINGS

Global Corruption Barometer 2010 New Zealand Results

AmericasBarometer Insights: 2011 Number 63

Retrospective Voting

Developing Political Preferences: Citizen Self-Interest

Constitutional Reform in California: The Surprising Divides

Web Appendix for More a Molehill than a Mountain: The Effects of the Blanket Primary on Elected Officials Behavior in California

The Cook Political Report / LSU Manship School Midterm Election Poll

Ai, C. and E. Norton Interaction Terms in Logit and Probit Models. Economic Letters

Supplementary/Online Appendix for:

CHAPTER 8 - POLITICAL PARTIES

Chapter 5. Residential Mobility in the United States and the Great Recession: A Shift to Local Moves

Issue Importance and Performance Voting. *** Soumis à Political Behavior ***

The Immigrant Double Disadvantage among Blacks in the United States. Katharine M. Donato Anna Jacobs Brittany Hearne

Compulsory versus Voluntary Voting Mechanisms: An Experimental Study

Volume 35, Issue 1. An examination of the effect of immigration on income inequality: A Gini index approach

public opinion & political behavior

Elite Polarization and Mass Political Engagement: Information, Alienation, and Mobilization

Dynamics of Indigenous and Non-Indigenous Labour Markets

Supplementary Materials A: Figures for All 7 Surveys Figure S1-A: Distribution of Predicted Probabilities of Voting in Primary Elections

Social Desirability and Response Validity: A Comparative Analysis of Overreporting Voter Turnout in Five Countries

Asymmetric Partisan Biases in Perceptions of Political Parties

Marist College Institute for Public Opinion Poughkeepsie, NY Phone Fax

Report for the Associated Press: Illinois and Georgia Election Studies in November 2014

UndecidedVotersinthe NovemberPresidential Election. anationalsurvey

A positive correlation between turnout and plurality does not refute the rational voter model

Household Income, Poverty, and Food-Stamp Use in Native-Born and Immigrant Households

PRIVATIZATION AND INSTITUTIONAL CHOICE

Partisan Nation: The Rise of Affective Partisan Polarization in the American Electorate

HISPANIC MEDIA SURVEY Topline - National

Appendix 1: Alternative Measures of Government Support

Talking Politics: Influences on Interpersonal Political Conversation. During the 2000 Election. Jennifer Myers. Nebraska Wesleyan University

Supplementary Material for Preventing Civil War: How the potential for international intervention can deter conflict onset.

Democratic theorists often turn to theories of

STEM CELL RESEARCH AND THE NEW CONGRESS: What Americans Think

Non-electoral Participation: Citizen-initiated Contact. and Collective Actions

Gender, age and migration in official statistics The availability and the explanatory power of official data on older BME women

FOR RELEASE APRIL 26, 2018

Author(s) Title Date Dataset(s) Abstract

Working Paper: The Effect of Electronic Voting Machines on Change in Support for Bush in the 2004 Florida Elections

AmericasBarometer Insights: 2010 (No.34) * Popular Support for Suppression of Minority Rights 1

Study Background. Part I. Voter Experience with Ballots, Precincts, and Poll Workers

Income Inequality as a Political Issue: Does it Matter?

What Can We Learn about Financial Access from U.S. Immigrants?

Where Do Presidents Politicize? Evidence from the George W. Bush Administration. David E. Lewis

Transcription:

A Report on the Social Network Battery in the 1998 American National Election Study Pilot Study Robert Huckfeldt Ronald Lake Indiana University January 2000 The 1998 Pilot Study of the American National Election Study included a very brief and economical social network battery which solicited information regarding the respondents' self identified networks of political discussion. In this report, and in the accompanying appendix, we explore the battery's analytic potential for understanding the influence of socially communicated political expertise. The ANES battery is based entirely on the perceptions of survey respondents regarding the characteristics of their identified discussants, without any validating information taken from the discussants themselves. We explore the utility of such a battery by comparing the ANES results to other studies that do include this validating information. The underlying question is whether it is analytically valuable to implement a social network battery based entirely on the respondent's perceptions. The ANES name generator was designed to provide information regarding the nature of the respondents' informally constructed networks at an affordable price measured in the currency of interviewing time. In an effort to characterize the breadth and depth of communication networks, respondents were asked only to provide the first names of discussants beyond the household. Closely held relationships are typically among those offered first by respondents to network batteries (Burt 1986), and hence the three-person network battery extends its reach by eliminating household discussants.

In addition to data regarding the size of the social network outside the household, the battery provides information on respondent perceptions regarding discussant voting preferences, as well as the respondent's ease in reporting these preferences. Of particular importance to this report, the battery also provides information regarding the respondents' perceptions regarding the frequency of political discussion with their first-named discussants, as well as respondents' assessments regarding the first discussants' knowledge of politics. Political Expertise and Networks of Communication Downs (1957) argued that the discussion of politics is an efficient way to minimize the information costs of meaningful political engagement. Sensible people search out well-informed associates with politically compatible viewpoints from whom to obtain information on the cheap (p. 219). The ANES social network battery allows an evaluation of Downs' argument. In addition to a political name generator that supplied up to three discussants outside the respondent's immediate household (see Part A of Table 1), the battery included two questions regarding the political expertise and frequency of political discussion with the first discussant named: 1 When you talk with [discussant #1] do you discuss political matters: - never - rarely - sometimes - often Generally speaking, how much do you think [discussant #1] knows about politics? - not much at all - an average amount - a great deal 1 The name generator used in the 1998 NES Pilot was, "From time to time, people discuss government, elections, and politics with other people. I'd like to know the people you talk with about these matters. Apart from the people in your immediate household, can you think of anyone?" If the response was yes, the interviewer solicited the first name of the discussant, and followed up with, "Is there anyone else you can think of?" for up to three discussants for each respondent outside the household.

Table 1B cross-tabulates the responses to these questions, showing that respondents are more likely to discuss politics frequently with discussants whom they judge to be politically expert. As it stands, this relationship is subject to a range of problems and questions. First, how do we know that the perceived expertise of the discussant has anything to do with objectively defined expertise? This question cannot be addressed directly based on ANES data because the pilot study does not include data taken from the discussants themselves. Rather, we employ a recent study that does include such information to examine the veracity of respondent perceptions regarding discussant expertise. This analysis, which is shown in the appendix, provides strong validation for the respondent perceptions (see appendix Table 4). The most important factors driving perceptions of discussant expertise are related to the actual levels of discussant expertise. Second, how important is the impact of perceived expertise on discussion frequency relative to a range of alternative factors that might predict political discussion? Most importantly, if politically knowledgeable citizens are more likely to discuss politics with others who are also knowledgeable, the effect of perceived expertise may be a spurious consequence of the main respondent's own knowledge and expertise. That is, those who are politically knowledgeable may be more likely to discuss politics with everyone, regardless of perceptions regarding expertise. We employ an ordered logit model in Table 2 to evaluate several competing explanations for the frequency of political discussion with the first-named discussant. In addition to the perceived expertise of the discussant and the actual expertise of the respondent, the model includes measures for the respondent's partisan extremity, education, age, and employment status. Finally, the model also includes a measure regarding the respondent's perceived

agreement with the discussant regarding the 1998 gubernatorial vote. Do people simply talk more to those with whom they agree, perhaps in an effort to avoid social discomfort? And are these perceptions of discussant expertise conditioned on agreement? The results show that it is the perceived level of political knowledge of the first discussant, along with the main respondent's objective level of political knowledge, that are statistically discernible in predicting the frequency of political discussion. 2 None of the other explanatory variables produce statistically discernible effects. How large are the effects due to the perceived knowledge of the discussant and the actual knowledge of the respondent? The magnitudes of both factors' effects on the predicted probability of frequent discussion are shown in Table 3. Both factors produce substantial effects, but the perceived expertise of the discussant is more influential in affecting discussion frequency. (These results mirror the parallel analysis undertaken in the appendix.) Before concluding, we turn briefly to a discussion of the lack of an effect due to perceived disagreement. Political Discussion Networks and Perceived Disagreement The respondents' perceptions of political agreement with their discussants do not produce a discernible effect in Table 2. This is perhaps surprising on both theoretical and substantive grounds. Both economic theory (Downs 1957) and cognitive dissonance theory (Festinger 1957) can be employed to produce an expectation that people would seek out agreeable associates with whom to discuss politics. Several explanations might account for the lack of such an effect. First, disagreement may be a rare and idiosyncratic event within these networks. Second, Calvert (1986) has argued that political information may be more valuable if it comes from 2 Ideally, we would conduct these analyses with the frequency of discussion about politics and the levels of political knowledge for all the discussants named by the respondent. In the 1998 NES Pilot Study, we are limited to data about the first respondent named.

someone with whom the receiver disagrees. Finally, we may be underestimating the incidence of political disagreement at the same time that we are overestimating its importance as a source of cognitive dissonance (Ross et al. 1973; Huckfeldt and Sprague 1999). This is not the place for a full examination of these issues, but it is important to address the frequency of perceived political disagreement within these networks before concluding the analysis. Disagreement regarding the gubernatorial candidates is not a rare event among the ANES respondents. Table 4A displays summary data regarding the correspondence between the respondents' reported preferences and their perceptions regarding the discussants' preferences. Levels of perceived disagreement range from 36 percent to 51 percent across the three discussants. These levels are, of course, even higher if we consider the level of political disagreement with at least one discussant. Indeed, disagreement with at least one discussant is the modal condition among the respondents. These levels of disagreement are somewhat higher than the levels of perceived disagreement taken from a post-election national survey of respondent-discussant dyads in the 1992 presidential election. In that study (Huckfeldt et al., 1995), 70 percent of the main respondents perceived agreement within a particular dyad. If we assume the likelihood of disagreement is independent across discussants, the probability of perceived political homogeneity in a three person network drops to.34. Several factors might account for the higher levels of disagreement in the ANES data. First, the disagreement measure is based on a gubernatorial election rather than a presidential election. Second, the ANES data does not include social relationships within the household that are likely to involve agreement. Third, the ANES data were taken before the election while the 1992 data are from a post-election study. Regardless of these relatively minor differences in

levels of disagreement, it is important to emphasize that neither the ANES pilot study nor any other study produces a picture of political homogeneity and agreement within social networks during an election campaign (Huckfeldt and Sprague 1999). Can we trust the respondent perceptions regarding agreement and disagreement with their discussants? Several analyses have shown systematic biases in the perception of other people's political preferences, created both by: (1) the political preferences of the perceiver and (2) the social setting of the perception (Huckfeldt and Sprague 1995, 1999; Huckfeldt et al. 1998). The impact of these biases should not be overestimated, however. Based on the 1992 national study, Part B of Table 4 shows that 91 percent of those who actually agree with their discussants correctly recognize the discussant's preference, and 63 percent of those who actually disagree with their discussants correctly recognize the discussant's preference. Several lessons can be taken from Table 4. First, perceived disagreement is widespread among the respondents' networks of political communication, and these levels of perceived disagreement modestly underestimate actual levels of disagreement. Second, while perceptions of disagreement are biased by the actual presence of agreement and disagreement, information regarding the size and nature of the bias is readily obtained from multiple sources. This information can be used in analyses where such information is not directly available where we do not have interview information with the discussants. Finally, the respondent's perception regarding a discussant is, for many purposes, more important than reality. For example, if we wish to assess the impact of disagreement on the frequency of discussion, it is arguably more important to know whether the respondent perceives disagreement with the discussant.

Conclusion The reality of citizenship is that individuals seldom go it alone when they engage in political activities. Preferences, choices, and levels of engagement are contingent on the location of individuals within particular social settings. The 1998 ANES Pilot Study pursued one strategy for the measurement of these settings the incorporation of a social network battery. Our own judgment is that such a strategy yields progress even when the network battery is not supplemented with survey data taken directly from the respondents. References Burt, Ronald S. 1986. "A Note on Sociometric Order in the General Social Survey Network Data," Social Networks 8:149-174. Calvert, Randall L. 1985. The Value of Biased Information: A Rational Choice Model of Political Advice, Journal of Politics 4: 530-555. Downs, Anthony. 1957. An Economic Theory of Democracy. New York: Harper and Row. Festinger, Leon. 1957. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press. Huckfeldt, Robert, Paul Allen Beck, Russell Dalton, and Jeffrey Levine. 1995. Political Environments, Cohesive Social Groups, and the Communication of Public Opinion, American Journal of Political Science 39: 1025-1054. Huckfeldt, Robert, Paul Allen Beck, Russell J. Dalton, Jeffrey Levine, and William Morgan. 1998. "Ambiguity, Distorted Messages, and Nested Environmental Effects on Political Communication," Journal of Politics 60: 996-1030. Huckfeldt, Robert and John Sprague. 1995. Citizens, Politics, and Social Communication. New York: Cambridge University Press. Robert Huckfeldt and John Sprague. 1999. "Political Environments, Micro-Environments, and the Dynamics of Political Preference." Prepared for delivery at the annual meeting American Political Science Association, Atlanta, September, 1999. Ross, Lee, Gunter Bierbauer, and Susan Hoffman. 1976. "The Role of Attribution Processes in Conformity and Dissent," American Psychologist 31: 148-157.

Table 1. Network Size and Frequency of Political Discussion. A. Number of discussant partners named by respondents using the 1998 ANES Pilot Name Generator. From time to time, people discuss government, elections, and politics with other people. I'd like to know the people you talk with about these matters. Apart from the people in your immediate household, can you think of anyone? Number of Discussants none 195 22.0% one 138 15.6 two 165 18.6 three 387 43.7 N= 885 B. Reported frequency of political discussion by perceived political expertise of discussant (first discussant named). When you talk with Generally speaking, how much do you think [discussant #1] do you [discussant #1] knows about politics? discuss political matters: not much at all an average amount a great deal often 9.4 21.8 49.6 sometimes 64.2 54.0 39.2 rarely 24.5 23.9 10.8 never 1.9% 0.3% 0.4% N= (679) 53 376 250 Source: American National Election Study, 1998 Pilot.

Table 2. Perceived frequency of political discussion (with the first discussant named) by perceived political knowledge of first discussant, main respondent political knowledge, partisan extremity of main respondent, perceived political agreement of main respondent with first discussant, and socioeconomic control variables. (Ordered logit model. T-values for coefficients and standard errors for cutting point thresholds are shown in parentheses.) coefficient (t-value) perceived first discussant political knowledge.9616 (7.434) main respondent knowledge.207 (4.452) main respondent partisan extremity.0553 (.643) perceived agreement regarding candidate preference -.1005 (.657) main respondent education -.0003 (.008) main respondent age -.0041 (.712) main respondent employment status.0298 (.162) threshold 1-2.6983 (s.e=.8359) threshold 2 1.3660 (s.e.=.617) threshold 3 3.7991 (s.e.=.6343) N= 673 chi 2 /df/p: 81.92/7/.00 pseudo R 2.06 perceived frequency of political discussion: 0=never, 1=rarely, 2=sometimes, 3=often perceived discussant political knowledge: 1=not much at all, 2=an average amount, 3= a great deal main respondent knowledge: sum of whether or not respondent named the Republican and Democratic gubernatorial candidates (0-2) plus four item knowledge battery (0-4), range 0-6 main respondent partisan extremity: 0=independent or non-partisan, 1=independent leaning toward Democrats or Republicans, 2=weak Republican or Democrat, 3=Strong Democrat or Republican perceived agreement of candidate preference: 1=respondent perceives that discussant gubernatorial candidate preference is the same as theirs, 0=otherwise main respondent education: years of school based on respondent self-report main respondent age: age in years based on respondent self-report main respondent employment status: 1=working now, 0=laid off, unemployed, retired, disabled, homemaker, or student Source: American National Election Study, 1998 Pilot.

Table 3. Predicted probability of frequent ("often") political discussion with discussant, across the range of respondent knowledge and perceived discussant knowledge. perceived discussant knowledge respondent knowledge not much at all a great deal lowest (0).04.24 highest (6).14.53 Source: Table 2 estimates. Partisan extremity is held constant at independent leaner. Perceived agreement is held constant at agree. Main respondent education is held constant at high school graduate. Main respondent age is held constant at 50 years. Employment status is held constant at currently working.

Table 4. Perceptions of Agreement and Disagreement with Discussants. A. Frequency of Perceived Disagreement with Discussant by Discussant Named. Source: American National Election Study, 1998 Pilot. Respondent Discussant #1 Discussant#2 Discussant#3 perceived agree 64% 49% 58% perceived disagree 36% 51% 42% N= 542 534 378 Source: American National Election Study, 1998 Pilot. B. Accuracy in Perception of Discussant's Votes by Agreement and Disagreement (All Respondent-Discussant Dyads). Source: 1992 American Cross-National Election Project. Main Respondent Perception is: Self Reported Votes of Discussant and Main Respondent are: Same Different Accurate 91.0% 63.5% Inaccurate 9.0% 36.5% N= 346 219 Source: Cross-National Election Project, 1992 American Study; Huckfeldt et al. 1995.