A Natural Experiment: Inadvertent Priming of Party Identification in a Split-Sample Survey

Similar documents
MOST NEW JERSEYANS SUPPORT CHRISTIE S APPEARANCE IN STORM ADS BUT THINK COMMERCIALS CREATORS CHOSEN FOR POLITICAL REASONS

U.S. Catholics split between intent to vote for Kerry and Bush.

PPIC Statewide Survey Methodology

Release #2475 Release Date: Wednesday, July 2, 2014 WHILE CALIFORNIANS ARE DISSATISFIED

These are the findings from the latest statewide Field Poll completed among 1,003 registered voters in early January.

POLL: CLINTON MAINTAINS BIG LEAD OVER TRUMP IN BAY STATE. As early voting nears, Democrat holds 32-point advantage in presidential race

Eagleton Institute of Politics Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey 191 Ryders Lane New Brunswick, New Jersey

Experiments in Election Reform: Voter Perceptions of Campaigns Under Preferential and Plurality Voting

THE FIELD POLL. By Mark DiCamillo, Director, The Field Poll

THE LOUISIANA SURVEY 2017

NEW JERSEYANS SEE NEW CONGRESS CHANGING COUNTRY S DIRECTION. Rutgers Poll: Nearly half of Garden Staters say GOP majority will limit Obama agenda

Opinion of North Carolina Voters on State Issues TABLE OF CONTENTS

Trump s Record, GOP Tax Bill May Suppress Republican Votes in Illinois

North Carolina Races Tighten as Election Day Approaches

North Carolina and the Federal Budget Crisis

UNEASE OVER THE WAR ON TERRORISM

MEDICAID EXPANSION RECEIVES BROAD SUPPORT CHRISTIE POSITIONED WELL AMONG ELECTORATE IMPROVES UPON FAVORABLES AMONG DEMOCRATS

THE FIELD POLL FOR ADVANCE PUBLICATION BY SUBSCRIBERS ONLY.

Public Remains Supportive of Israel, Wary of Iran

Opinion about North Carolina Political Leaders: One Year after Election 2016 TABLE OF CONTENTS

Poll shows Carper, Blunt Rochester way out in front Big leads by Democrats consistent with forecasted Blue Wave

2018 Florida General Election Poll

Eagleton Institute of Politics Rutgers University New Brunswick 191 Ryders Lane New Brunswick, New Jersey

State Governments Viewed Favorably as Federal Rating Hits New Low

TIME FOR A WOMAN IN THE OVAL OFFICE? NEW JERSEYANS AGREE COUNTRY IS READY

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, May, 2017, Partisan Identification Is Sticky, but About 10% Switched Parties Over the Past Year

THE LOUISIANA SURVEY 2018

NUMBERS, FACTS AND TRENDS SHAPING THE WORLD FOR RELEASE AUGUST 25, 2014 FOR FURTHER INFORMATION ON THIS REPORT:

Minnesota Public Radio News and Humphrey Institute Poll. Dayton Jumps to Double-Digit Lead Over Emmer

THE LOUISIANA SURVEY 2017

Voters More Optimistic About Direction of State; Support Reforms, Wage Hike Proposal

BLISS INSTITUTE 2006 GENERAL ELECTION SURVEY

Illinois Voters are Not Happy with the Direction of the State: Not Much Influenced by the Recent Tax Cuts

IMMEDIATE RELEASE DECEMBER 22, 2014

Report for the Associated Press: Illinois and Georgia Election Studies in November 2014

NH Statewide Horserace Poll

THE GOVERNOR, THE PRESIDENT, AND SANDY GOOD NUMBERS IN THE DAYS AFTER THE STORM

1. The Relationship Between Party Control, Latino CVAP and the Passage of Bills Benefitting Immigrants

Job approval in North Carolina N=770 / +/-3.53%

HIGH POINT UNIVERSITY POLL MEMO RELEASE 9/24/2018 (UPDATE)

Results Embargoed Until Tuesday, April 24, 2018 at 12:01am

SURVEY KEY FINDINGS. Require RPS of 20 percent by 2020

FOR RELEASE October 1, 2018

NUMBERS, FACTS AND TRENDS SHAPING THE WORLD. FOR RELEASE September 12, 2014 FOR FURTHER INFORMATION ON THIS REPORT:

November 15-18, 2013 Open Government Survey

Jim Justice Leads in Race for West Virginia Governor

Release #2337 Release Date and Time: 6:00 a.m., Friday, June 4, 2010

Results Embargoed Until Wednesday, February 24 at 12:01AM. Clinton Continues to Lead in Maryland; Edwards and Van Hollen in Dead Heat

RUTGERS-EAGLETON POLL: NEW JERSEYANS SAY KEEP MENENDEZ IN OFFICE UNLESS PROVEN GUILTY

How Well Do We Understand Sexual Harassment?

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, July, 2014, Most Think the U.S. Has No Responsibility to Act in Iraq

STATE GIVES THUMBS UP TO GOVERNOR CHALLENGERS FACE AN UPHILL BATTLE IN 2013

Online Appendix 1: Treatment Stimuli

Record Number Favors Removing U.S. Troops from Afghanistan

FOR RELEASE DECEMBER 14, 2017

Support for Restoring U.S.-Cuba Relations March 11-15, 2016

FEDERAL VOTING PREFERENCES IN MANITOBA

GOP Seen as Principled, But Out of Touch and Too Extreme

Trump and Sanders Have Big Leads in MetroNews West Virginia Poll

Greenberg Quinlan Rosner/Democracy Corps

Release #2345 Release Date: Tuesday, July 13, 2010

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, February 2014, Public Divided over Increased Deportation of Unauthorized Immigrants

Learning from Small Subsamples without Cherry Picking: The Case of Non-Citizen Registration and Voting

MEREDITH COLLEGE POLL September 18-22, 2016

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, September 2014, Growing Public Concern about Rise of Islamic Extremism At Home and Abroad

PRRI March 2018 Survey Total = 2,020 (810 Landline, 1,210 Cell) March 14 March 25, 2018

Results Embargoed Until Monday, September 25, 2017 at 12:01am

Party Cue Inference Experiment. January 10, Research Question and Objective

COMMUNITY RESILIENCE STUDY

Telephone Survey. Contents *

Marist College Institute for Public Opinion Poughkeepsie, NY Phone Fax

Eagleton Institute of Politics Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey 191 Ryders Lane New Brunswick, New Jersey

Supporting Information for Do Perceptions of Ballot Secrecy Influence Turnout? Results from a Field Experiment

UNIVERSITY OF MASSACHUSETTS LOWELL MASSACHUSETTS U.S. SENATE POLL Sept , ,005 Registered Voters (RVs)

FOR RELEASE AUGUST 4, 2017

CONTACTS: MURRAY EDELMAN, Ph.D., (917) (cell) TIM VERCELLOTTI, Ph.D., (732) , EXT. 285; (919) (cell)

NBC News/WSJ/Marist Poll

REGISTERED VOTERS October 30, 2016 October 13, 2016 Approve Disapprove Unsure 7 6 Total

PEW RESEARCH CENTER. FOR RELEASE January 16, 2019 FOR MEDIA OR OTHER INQUIRIES:

Public Opinion on Health Care Issues

Marist College Institute for Public Opinion Poughkeepsie, NY Phone Fax

Concerns About a Terrorist Attack in the U.S. Rise November 19-22, 2015

How the Survey was Conducted Nature of the Sample: NBC News/Marist Poll of 1,033 Michigan Adults

Author(s) Title Date Dataset(s) Abstract

Tony Licciardi Department of Political Science

Obama Maintains Approval Advantage, But GOP Runs Even on Key Issues

THE PUBLIC AND THE CRITICAL ISSUES BEFORE CONGRESS IN THE SUMMER AND FALL OF 2017

Colorado TABOR: A Survey of Colorado Likely Voters Age 18+ Data Collected by Alan Newman Research, Inc. Report Prepared by Joanne Binette

Marist College Institute for Public Opinion Poughkeepsie, NY Phone Fax

PRRI/The Atlantic April 2016 Survey Total = 2,033 (813 Landline, 1,220 Cell phone) March 30 April 3, 2016

VOTERS APPROVE OF GAY MARRIAGE DECISION; BOOKER AND CHRISTIE REMAIN IN THE LEAD

None of the Top Political Leaders Get High Marks from Illinois Voters

Rick Santorum has erased 7.91 point deficit to move into a statistical tie with Mitt Romney the night before voters go to the polls in Michigan.

EMBARGOED NOT FOR RELEASE UNTIL: SUNDAY, OCTOBER 17, 1993 FLORIO MAINTAINS LEAD OVER WHITMAN; UNFAVORABLE IMPRESSIONS OF BOTH CANDIDATES INCREASE

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, October, 2016, Trump, Clinton supporters differ on how media should cover controversial statements

CONTACTS: MURRAY EDELMAN, Ph.D., (917) (cell) TIM VERCELLOTTI, Ph.D., (732) , EXT. 285; (919) (cell)

Presidential Race Nip and Tuck in Michigan

IN POLITICS, WHAT YOU KNOW IS LESS IMPORTANT THAN WHAT YOU D LIKE TO BELIEVE

About IVR Surveys Post-Weighting

RECOMMENDED CITATION: Pew Research Center, May, 2015, Negative Views of New Congress Cross Party Lines

Transcription:

Vol. 8, Issue 6, 2015 A Natural Experiment: Inadvertent Priming of Party Identification in a Split-Sample Survey Marc D. Weiner * * Institution: Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey Department: Bloustein School of Planning and Public Policy

Abstract The research question is whether, and to what degree, questionnaire assignment in a split-sample survey may have affected the respondent s self-report of party identification. We observe that the degree of Republican party identification can be inflated by priming with typically conservative issue concerns, such as the public economics of disaster recovery and rebuilding, and the role of the state in regulating development. At the same time, we saw that Democratic party identification is immune to such priming, but can be inflated by priming exposure to traditional liberal and more affective concerns, here personal experiences of the respondent up to, during, and immediately following Superstorm Sandy. Survey Practice 1

Introduction The 2013 Rutgers-Abt SRBI New Jersey Hurricane Impact Survey collected data from 1,751 telephone respondents via a random-digit-dial probability sample of non-institutionalized adults living in New Jersey (1,138 landline and 613 cell phone). The survey was fielded from February 15 to March 27, 2013, about three-and-a-half months after Superstorm Sandy hit New Jersey. 1 Administered in English and Spanish (3.5 percent of all interviews), the sampling design was a modified split ballot with all respondents going through a screening and preliminary set of questions, followed by a randomized sample split for the bulk of the content of questionnaire; near the conclusion of the survey, the sample was rejoined for the demography section. We observed a clear inadvertent priming effect in the distribution of party identification based on which version of the questionnaire the respondent received. This was true whether we assessed major party identification only or since New Jersey permits registration as Unaffiliated in various configurations including self-reported Independent on its own, as well as in combination with the self-reported voluntary answer, no preference. The practical lesson suggests that under a split-sample design priming effects analyses should be conducted. The remainder of this note demonstrates a simple and effective approach to conduct that analysis, as well as to treat the priming effect as an inadvertent survey experiment. Methods Priming effects are generated when prior items in a questionnaire affect the answer value distribution of subsequent items (Van de Walle and Van Ryzin 2011; citing Tourangeau et al. 2000). More broadly, something about questionnaire design makes a concept more likely to come to mind and therefor increases the impact of that concept on other cognitive processes (Cassino and Erisen 1 Calculated under the American Association for Public Opinion Research (AAPOR) RR3 approach, response rates were 18.6 percent for the landline sample and 17.0 percent for the cell sample, which yields a combined overall response rate of 17.8 percent. Cooperation rates, using the AAPOR COOP3 approach, were calculated at 35.9 percent for the landline sample and 33.2 percent for the cell phone sample, yielding a combined overall cooperation rate of 34.6 percent (AAPOR 2015, pp. 53 54). Survey Practice 2

2010, p. 375; for a more extensive discussion of priming generally, see Weiner et al. 2013, pp. 192 193). Our questionnaire was split such that one-half of the sample received an experiential version collecting data focusing on the respondent s personal and household experiences during and immediately following the storm. The other half received an economic version focusing on the budgetary and regulatory aspects of New Jersey s recovery from the Superstorm s extensive damage to residences, commercial buildings, and public infrastructure. The experiential version activated recollections of the storm (and, if applicable, evacuation) experience, including concerns for the safety of life and property. The economic version activated current assessments and future projections of taxation, regulation, limits on development, specifically invoking government as a stakeholder and actor. We did not, however, anticipate that these primes would affect respondents self-reports of their party identification. The party identification question we used was the most basic National Election Study format: Generally speaking, do you usually think of yourself as a Republican, a Democrat, an Independent, or what? (American National Election Studies 2010). Precodes included Republican, Democrat, independent, while voluntary responses of Tea Party, Green Party, other/ specify, no preference, don t know, and refused were permitted. In and around 2013, the distribution of party identification in New Jersey was approximately 34 percent Republican and 46 percent Democrat, including leaners (Gallup 2014). Results From the full sample of 1,751, we first analyzed the 939 respondents who reported identification with one of the two major parties. Since the split sampling was randomly assigned, the distribution across the by-version subsamples of Republican and Democratic Party identification should have been random and, generally speaking, should have aligned with the Gallup estimates. Survey Practice 3

Table 1, however, tells a different story. Fifty-five percent of respondents exposed to the economic version of the questionnaire identified as Republicans, 21 percentage points higher than the Gallup statewide estimate and nearly 10 percentage points (or 22 percent) greater than those exposed to the experiential questionnaire. Table 1 Distribution of self-reported major party identification by questionnaire version. Economic version Experiential version Row totals Republican 54.9% (n=192) 45.1% (n=158) 100% (n=350) Democrats 45.7% (n=269) 54.3% (n=320) 100% (n=589) Total 49.1% (n=461) 50.9% (n=478) 100% (n=939) The Democrats row in Table 1 is even more interesting: Respondents exposed to the economic issues version of the questionnaire self-reported Democratic Party identification at nearly spot-on the Gallup statewide estimate of 46 percent. The inference, of course, is that the Gallup measure is likely a solid indicator of Democratic Party identification, i.e., that it is of sufficient permanence to not respond to the priming effect of the economic issues version of the survey. It seems, in New Jersey at least, that self-reported Republican Party identification is either more malleable generally, or more subject to activation upon priming. However, when exposed to the adverse weather experiential questionnaire versions, respondents self-reported as Democrats at nearly 9 percentage points higher, or about 17 percent greater than the Gallup estimate, than they did after exposure to the economic questionnaire. On the face of it, then, it seems that the degree of self-reported Republican Party identification in New Jersey can be artificially inflated via priming through exposure to economic and regulatory concerns, which invoke the power of the state. The companion self-reported Democratic Party identification is less subject to being diluted or strengthened through such priming, and is less related to the presence or absence of the state as an actor in the context of the Superstorm Sandy experience. Democratic Party identification is, however, apparently subject to empathetic priming, here through probes that explore the suffering of those personally and directly affected Survey Practice 4

by Superstorm Sandy. We then ran the same analysis this time including all three political identification precodes, i.e., Independent in addition to Republican and Democrat. New Jersey is somewhat unique in permitting registration as Unaffiliated, which we take as the functional equivalent of political party independence. This opportunity for the rejection of partisanship, while remaining in the partisan game, generates a statewide political culture under which the non-aligned (i.e., non-identifiers) are treated as identifiers of a sort when assessing electoral outcomes. With the functional equivalent of registered Independents included the number of cases under analysis increased by 576, to 1,515, while still operating under the theoretical expectation that the distribution of party identification and registered independent status should be random across all three groups. Table 2 shows the added results for independents who show an approximately 6 percentage point (i.e., 13 percent) difference. Table 2 Distribution of self-reported major party identification and political independence by questionnaire version. Economic version Experiential version Row totals Republican 54.9% (n=192) 45.1% (n=158) 100% (n=350) Democrats 45.7% (n=269) 54.3% (n=320) 100% (n=589) Independents 53.0% (n=305) 47.1% (n=271) 100% (n=576) Total 50.6% (n=766) 49.4% (n=749) 100% (n=1,515) We assessed one last crosstabulation, here including as a substantive category 119 respondents who reported no preference in response to the party identification probe. Adding this group in brought the number of cases under analysis up to 1,634; these expanded results are shown in Table 3. Survey Practice 5

Table 3 Distribution of self-reported major party identification, political independence, and no political preference by questionnaire version. Economic version Experiential version Row totals Republican 54.9% (n=192) 45.1% (n=158) 100% (n=350) Democrats 45.7% (n=269) 54.3% (n=320) 100% (n=589) Independents 53.0% (n=305) 47.1% (n=271) 100% (n=576) No Preference 45.4% (n=54) 54.6% (n=65) 100% (n=119) Total 50.2% (n=820) 49.8% (n=814) 100% (n=1,634) In light of this now full picture, apparently natural groupings emerge: (1) those with no preference seem to respond to the experiential priming in a way very similar to Democrats; while, (2) Independents, however, seem to run in closer parallel to Republicans. This seemed, to us, to be something worth investigating. An Inadvertent, If not Natural, al, Survey Experiment We were interested in learning if respondents primed by a questionnaire exploring economic and regulatory issues were more likely to self-report as Republicans, which respondents were most susceptible to the prime, and, in which direction? On the face of it, it seems they are drawn from the pool that would otherwise have self-reported as Independent. The same pattern holds for Democrats, except there the source of the inflation is from those who it seems would otherwise have responded no preference. To better explore this effect of economic issue priming on self-reported partisan affiliation, we constructed one multinomial logit model. The predictors are (1) the version of the questionnaire to which the respondent was exposed; (2) education; and (3) age; the last two entered in intervals. The model was run twice, first with the three categories Republican, Democrat, and no preference, compared against the base category Independent, then with Republican, Democrat, and Independent, compared against the base category no preference. By examining these outcomes where only the comparison base outcome varied between the model runs we can get insight into the direct effect of the sole essential independent variable, i.e., economic issue priming, on self-reported party identification. Survey Practice 6

In particular, we show how such economic issue priming enhances the likelihood that an otherwise self-reported Independent will report Republican Party identification, and, in turn, inflate the reported proportion of Republican Party identifiers in the sample. We report the substantive outcomes in terms of relative risk ratios, 2 which report the risk of occurrence of a characteristic in the group of interest, relative to the base group. As shown in Table 4, the multinomial logit model performs as expected: When compared to Independents, Democrats are only about 28 percent as likely to be affected (0.72, p=0.005) by economic priming. Under the same comparison, Republicans are unaffected (1.07, p=0.631), indicating a similarity in response to Independents. Table 4 Multinomial logit assessing affect of economic issue priming on reporting of party identification by political independents and No Preference identifiers. Model fit: n=1,624 LR χ2 (21)=77.72 p=0.0000 LL= 2001.2535 Compared to base outcome Independent Relative e risk ratio Std. Err. Z P> z Republican, effect of economic priming 1.07 0.0147 0.48 0.631 Democrat, effect of economic priming 0.72 0.0853 2.78 0.005 No Preference, effect of economic priming 0.73 0.1497 1.55 0.122 Compared to base outcome No Preference Relative e risk ratio Std. Err. Z P> z Republican, effect of economic priming 1.47 0.3186 1.77 0.077 Democrat, effect of economic priming 0.99 0.2022 0.06 0.953 Independent, effect of economic priming 1.37 0.2826 1.55 0.122 When compared to those with no preference, Republicans are about 47 percent more likely to be affected by economic priming (1.47, p=0.077), while Democrats are unaffected (0.99, p=0.953) indicating a similarity in response to the group with no preference. 2 Relative risk ratios are more intuitive than odds ratios. Under an odds ratio an analyst has to comprehend and explain what it means for one group to be more likely or less likely than another group to show a characteristic. Under relative risk ratios, one group is fixed as the comparison point. For an extensive discussion of the proper interpretation of these metrics, see Weiner et al. 2012, p. 189, footnotes 10 and 11. Survey Practice 7

Moreover, we conducted a postestimation likelihood ratio test comparing the full model with the variable controlling for questionnaire version included with the nested model omitting that variable. The outcome of that test [LR χ2 (3)=12.32; p=0.0064], rejects the null hypothesis that questionnaire version (here, with economic issues) does not affect the self-reporting of partisan identification. Finally, we note two limitations of this research. First, our measure of party identification is limited. Given the circumstances in which we found ourselves, we suspect we could have learned more with a five- or seven-point scale to capturing party identification. Second, our multinomial model is no by no means a full exploration of the determination of party identification. Rather, it is intended solely to confirm the suspected priming effect and, for that reason, only the most basic additional predictors education and age were included. Conclusion The implications for the interpretation of split-sample surveys, especially those with an economic questionnaire version, are clear: In most cases, the demographic component of a survey is at the end. Survey researchers should be cautious as to the impact content will have on self-reports of party identification. In interpreting such findings, researchers should recognize that inferences on the impact of priming on self-reported party identification must be considered in light of state election law. New Jersey s unique allowance that independents can register as Unaffiliated voters may, in the survey setting, generate inconsistent de jure and de facto understandings of the word independent. Acknowledgements Grateful acknowledgement for direct and indirect funding goes to James W. Hughes, Dean of the Edward J. Bloustein School of Planning and Public Policy and Robert M. Goodman, Dean of the School of Biological and Environmental Sciences, both of Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey; the Rockefeller Brothers Fund; the Mineta National Transit Research Consortium, Survey Practice 8

administered by Robert B. Noland of the Alan M. Voorhees Transportation Center at the Edward J. Bloustein School of Planning and Public Policy at Rutgers University; and Abt SRBI, administered by Mark Schulman and Chintan Turakhia. M. Patrick Simon and K. T. G. Weiner-Simon provided ongoing support and encouragement. References American Association for Public Opinion Research. 2015. Standard definitions: final dispositions of case codes and outcome rates for surveys. 8th ed. AAPOR, Oakbrook Terrace, IL. American National Election Studies. 2010. The ANES Guide to public opinion and electoral behavior. University of Michigan, Center for Political Studies, Ann Arbor, MI. Available at: www.el ectionstudies.org. Cassino, D. and C. Erisen. 2010. Priming Bush and Iraq in 2008: A survey experiment. American Politics Research 38(2): 372 394. Gallup. State of the States. 2014. Available at: http://www.gallup.c om/poll/125066/state-states.aspx. Tourangeau, R., L.J. Rips and K. Rasinski. 2000. The Psychology of Survey Responses. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA. Van de Walle, S. and G.G. Van Ryzin. 2011. The order of questions in a survey on citizen satisfaction with public services: lessons from a split-ballot experiment. Public Administration 89(4): 1436 1450. Weiner, M.D., O.T. Puniello, R.B. Noland, D. Ciemnecki and C. Turakhia. 2012. Consider the non-adopter: developing a prediction model for the adoption of household-level broadband access. Socio-Economic Planning Sciences: The International Journal of Public Sector Decision-Making 46(3): 183 193. Weiner, M.D., T.D. MacKinnon and M.R. Greenberg. 2013. Exploring the gender gap and the impact of residential location on environmental risk tolerance. Journal of Environmental Survey Practice 9

Psychology 36 (Dec.): 190 201. Survey Practice 10